This paper was converted on www.awesomepapers.org from LaTeX by an anonymous user.
Want to know more? Visit the Converter page.

Adaptive survival movement strategy to local epidemic outbreaks in cyclic models

J. Menezes1,2, B. Moura3,4 and E. Rangel2 1 Institute for Biodiversity and Ecosystem Dynamics, University of Amsterdam, Science Park 904, 1098 XH Amsterdam, The Netherlands 2 School of Science and Technology, Federal University of Rio Grande do Norte
59072-970, P.O. Box 1524, Natal, RN, Brazil
3 Edmond and Lily Safra International Institute of Neuroscience, Santos Dumont Institute, Av Santos Dumont 1560, 59280-000, Macaiba, RN, Brazil 4 Department of Biomedical Engineering, Federal University of Rio Grande do Norte, Av. Senador Salgado Filho 300, Lagoa Nova, 59078-970, Natal, RN, Brazil [email protected]
Abstract

We study the generalised rock-paper-scissors game with five species whose organisms face local epidemic outbreaks. As an evolutionary behavioural survival strategy, organisms of one out of the species move in the direction with more enemies of their enemies to benefit from protection against selection. We consider that each organism scans the environment, performing social distancing instead of agglomerating when perceiving that the density of sick organisms is higher than a tolerable threshold. Running stochastic simulations, we study the interference of the adaptive movement survival strategy in spatial pattern formation, calculating the characteristic length scale of the typical spatial domains inhabited by organisms of each species. We compute how social distancing trigger impacts the chances of an individual being killed in the cyclic game and contaminated by the disease. The outcomes show that the species predominates in the cyclic game because of the organisms’ local adaptation. The territory occupied by the species grows with the proportion of individuals learning to trigger the social distancing tactic. We also show that organisms that perceive large distances more properly execute the adaptive strategy, promptly triggering the social distancing tactic and choosing the correct direction to move. Our findings may contribute to understanding the role of adaptive behaviour when environmental changes threaten biodiversity.

1 Introduction

Spatial interactions among species play a vital role in the stability of ecosystems [1, 2, 3]. It has been reported that many species evolve to better adapt to the local environmental conditions, increasing the chances of controlling the natural resources to ensure species persistence [4, 5]. Motivated by local stimuli, many animals move strategically as an adaptive response to alterations in their living conditions [6, 7, 8, 9, 10, 11, 12]. Following environmental cues, organisms can escape threats to their existence, making alliances with other species to protect them against natural enemies’ attacks. Furthermore, scanning the neighbourhood helps individuals observe additional survival hazards ravaging the ecosystem, such as epidemic outbreaks [13, 14, 15, 16, 17, 18]. Using sensory information, individuals can decide when it is decisive to flee from enemies or perform social distancing strategies [19, 20, 21].

There is plenty of evidence that self-preservation behavioural strategies play a central role in controlling natural resources. Therefore, prioritising moving toward the direction where the probability of being killed is minimum has been demonstrated to be more advantageous in cyclic game models [22, 23, 24, 25, 26]. It has been shown in Ref.  [22] that the Safeguard strategy is advantageous for species participating in a cyclic spatial game, giving the species the possession of the most significant fraction of the territory. However, as investigated in Ref. [27], in the event of an epidemic strikes the system, individuals’ concern about survival increases: besides being eliminated in the cyclic spatial game, they are vulnerable to being contaminated by the infectious disease that is transmitted person-to-person. In this scenario, the Safeguard tactic, which involves a social approach, may become problematic since the organism must be distant from all possible viral vectors. To avoid contamination, thus, organisms perform the Social Distancing strategy, moving toward the direction with the highest density of empty species [27]. But neglecting the protection against enemies to perform the Social Distancing strategy is a dilemma that needs to be well considered by each organism. Combining both survival movement strategies provides better protection because if executing only Social Distancing, the individual is vulnerable to the attack of enemies [27].

In this work, instead of a fixed global design for the combination of survival movement strategies as investigated in Ref. [27], valid for all organisms in the system, each individual scans the neighbourhood to observe the presence of sick individuals. This allows the individual to decide which survival strategy to perform at each instant, switching the self-preservation strategy in response to a change in the local living conditions. Epidemic outbreaks can reach departed regions differently because the pathogen that causes the disease is passed from individual to individual. Thus, each organism needs to be aware of the arrival of new disease waves in the local community. If the chances of being infected are higher than a tolerable threshold, Social Distancing is executed; otherwise, the Safeguard tactic is continued. The decision is based on the individual’s capacity to perceive the neighbourhood: i) how far the organisms can scan their neighbourhood; ii) the cognitive and adaptive ability to interpret the signals received and change from the Safeguard to Social Distancing strategy when facing a local epidemic outbreak.

Our goal is to understand how the adaptive survival strategy to local epidemic outbreaks modifies the pattern formation process and changes the average size of the typical spatial domains. Then, we aim to quantify the benefits of behavioural tactics in the organisms’ selection and infection risks. Our simulations consider various tolerable thresholds for organisms to trigger the Social Distancing strategy. The research is performed using stochastic simulations based on the May-Leonard implementation of the rock-paper-scissors model, where the total number of individuals is not conserved [28, 29, 30, 31, 32, 33, 34, 35, 36, 37]. The disease spreading happens between two immediate neighbours in the lattice, with a sick individual transmitting the virus to a healthy organism, irrespective of the species [27, 38, 39, 40]. Sick organisms act as viral vectors, with a probability of being cured or dying, according to the disease virulence; no organism or species is immune, even after being cured of the disease.

The outline of this paper is as follows. The methods are described in Sec. 2, where we introduce the model and the simulation implementation. Next, we explore the role of the locally adaptive survival movement tactic in the spatial patterns and calculate the dynamics of the densities of species in Sec. 3. In Sec. 4, we compute the impact of the local strategies on organisms’ selection and infection risks; the densities of species and the characteristic scale of the typical areas occupied by each species in terms of the threshold are also computed. The dependence of the outcomes on the individuals’ physical abilities to perform the survival strategy is quantified in Sec. 6. Finally, we discuss the results and present our conclusions in Sec. 7.

Refer to caption
Figure 1: Illustration of the generalised rock-paper-scissors model with 55 species. Selection interactions are represented by arrows indicating the dominance of organisms of species ii over individuals of species i+1i+1.

2 Methods

2.1 The model

We investigate a cyclic game system composed of five species whose selection interactions follow the generalised rock-paper-scissors model, illustrated in Fig. 1. Accordingly, individuals of species ii dominate over individuals of species i+1i+1, with i=1,2,3,4,5i=1,2,3,4,5; we assume the identification i=i+5αi=i+5\,\alpha, where α\alpha is an integer. We consider that an epidemic spreads through the system, with organisms of all species equally susceptible to infection and reinfection by a disease transmitted person to person. All infected organisms become viral vectors to contaminate their immediate neighbours; some sick individuals die while others are cured.

In our model, organisms of one out of the species perform behavioural movement strategies to protect themselves against nearby enemies and infection by approaching viral vectors. Individuals usually perform the Safeguard strategy, moving towards the direction with the highest density of enemies of their enemies [22]. Therefore, they profit from being close to guards, decreasing the risk of being killed in the cyclic game. However, if the density of sick individuals in the vicinity represents a danger of imminent contamination, the individual opts for the Social Distancing tactic [27]. The decision to move to the areas with lower population density is individual, being triggered every time a surge affects the organism’s neighbourhood making the local density of sick individuals higher than a given threshold.

2.2 Simulations

We use square lattices with periodic boundary conditions to implement our stochastic simulations. As the spatial interactions may lead to organisms’ births and deaths, we use the May-Leonard implementation: the total number of individuals is not conserved [41]. We assume that each grid point contains at most one individual, which limits the maximum number of individuals to equal the total number of grid points, 𝒩\mathcal{N}. The densities of individuals of species ii at time tt is defined as ρi=Ii/𝒩\rho_{i}=I_{i}/\mathcal{N}, where IiI_{i} is number of individuals of species ii, with i=1,2,3,4,5i=1,2,3,4,5.

We identify the organisms using the notation hih_{i} and sis_{i} for healthy and sick individuals; the use of the identification ii stands for individuals irrespective of illness or health, with i=1,2,3,4,5i=1,2,3,4,5. This notation allows the description of the spatial interactions as [27]:

  • Selection: ijii\ j\to i\ \otimes\,, with j=i+1j=i+1, where \otimes means an empty space; selection interactions happens between individuals, irrespective of the healthy conditions;

  • Reproduction: iihii\ \otimes\to i\ h_{i}\,; a reproduction interaction generates a new healthy individual.

  • Mobility: iii\ \odot\to\odot\ i\,, where \odot means either an individual of any species or an empty space; when moving, a healthy or a sick individual switches position with another organism of any species or with an empty site;

  • Infection: sihjsisjs_{i}\ h_{j}\to s_{i}\ s_{j}\,, with i,j=1,2,3,4,5i,j=1,2,3,4,5: a sick individual passes the disease to another organism of any species;

  • Cure: sihis_{i}\to h_{i}\,: an ill individual is naturally cured of the disease without gaining immunity, thus being subject to reinfection;

  • Death: sis_{i}\to\otimes\,: when a sick organism dies because of the disease, its position is left empty.

Empty spaces are created when individuals are selected, or sick organisms die because of the disease severity.

The spatial interactions are executed by assuming the von Neumann neighbourhood: individuals may interact with one of their four immediate neighbours. At each time step, one of the possible implementations is chosen aleatory, according to the respective interaction probabilities: i) healthy individuals: shs_{h} (selection), rhr_{h} (reproduction), mhm_{h} (mobility); ii) sick organisms: sss_{s} (selection), rsr_{s} (reproduction), msm_{s} (mobility), ω\omega (infection), cc (cure), and dd (death by disease complications). The parameters are the same for all organisms of every species. Our algorithm follows three steps: i) randomly selecting an active individual; ii) using the model probabilities to choose one interaction to be executed; iii) drawing one of the four nearest neighbours to suffer the action, except for the directional movement strategy, where the organism chooses the direction to move according to the specific survival strategy. If the interaction is implemented, one timestep is counted. Otherwise, the three steps are redone. We assume the time unit called generation, defined as the necessary time to 𝒩\mathcal{N} timesteps to occur.

2.3 Adaptive survival strategy to local epidemic outbreaks

We investigate a locally adaptive self-preservation strategy of individuals that use environmental cues to move cleverly according to the current survival conditions. This means that each organism interprets the signals received from the neighbourhood to identify the existence of local disease surges, thus maximising the protection from disease infection and enemies’ attacks.

Each organism usually moves to minimise the chances of being caught and eliminated in the spatial competition game, performing the Safeguard strategy - this is independent of the organism’s geographic position [22, 27]. When performing the Safeguard strategy, an individual of species ii moves in the direction with the highest concentration of individuals of species i2i-2. This provides protection against individuals of species i1i-1, according to Fig. 1. However, the Safeguard tactic increases the chances of contamination during a local epidemic since the virus is transmitted from individual to individual. Because of this, organisms adapt to respond to the arrival of a local disease surge, temporarily switching from the Safeguard to the Social Distancing strategy, moving towards the direction with more empty spaces.

Every time an organism decides to perform the Social Distancing instead of Safeguard strategy, the vulnerability to being killed in the spatial game increases. Therefore, each organism scans the vicinity to conclude how to move. This means that the choice of the survival strategy is local, based on each organism’s reality. We define a real parameter β\beta, with 0β 10\,\leq\,\beta\,\leq\,1, to define the social distancing trigger parameter: a threshold assumed by the organisms as the minimum local density of sick individuals to perform the Social Distancing strategy. Only if the local density of sick individuals is higher than β\beta, the Social Distancing is performed. Otherwise, the Safeguard tactic is executed.

2.4 The adaptation factor

Adapting the survival movement strategy also depends on the organisms’ conditioning, meaning their cognitive ability to learn how to react to the presence of sick individuals. We then introduce the adaptation factor, λ\lambda, a real parameter, 0λ10\leq\lambda\leq 1, which indicates the organism’s conditioning to respond to a local surge appropriately. For λ=0\lambda=0, the organism cannot identify the presence of sick individuals, performing only the Safeguard strategy. In the limit λ=1\lambda=1, the organism is fully conditioned to react to the local surges correctly.

Every time an individual of species 11 is stochastically chosen to move, the adaptation factor defines the probability of the survival movement strategy being implemented. Namely, λ\lambda is defined as the probability of the organism being able to execute the behavioural strategy; 1λ1-\lambda is the probability of moving randomly.

2.5 Implementation of the survival movement strategies

The numerical implementation of the strategic movement follows the steps [33, 27]:

  1. 1.

    We define the perception radius RR, measured in lattice spacing, as the maximum distance an individual can scan the environmental cues to use the information to decide what strategic movement is suitable at the moment.

  2. 2.

    We implement a circular area of radius \mathcal{R} centred in the active individual, defining the total grid points the organism is able to analyse.

  3. 3.

    We calculate the density of sick organisms within the disc of radius RR surrounding the active individual. In case of the local density is below the threshold β\beta, the organism decides to perform the Safeguard strategy; otherwise, the Social Distancing tactic is chosen.

  4. 4.

    We separate the organism’s perception area into four circular sectors in the directions of the nearest neighbour. Thus, we count the number of empty spaces and organisms within each circular sector; individuals on the borders are assumed to be in both circular sectors.

  5. 5.

    We define the movement direction according to the organism’s strategy choice: the direction with the larger number of organisms of species i2i-2 (hi2+si2h_{i-2}+s_{i-2}) for Safeguard or the direction with more empty spaces for Social Distancing. If more than one direction is equally attractive, a draw is done.

2.6 Spatial Autocorrelation Function

To study the effects of the adaptive survival strategy to local epidemic outbreaks in the spatial patterns, we calculate the characteristic length scale of the typical spatial domains occupied by each species. For this purpose, we compute the spatial autocorrelation function Ci(r)C_{i}(r), with i=1,2,3,4,5i=1,2,3,4,5, in terms of radial coordinate rr. Defining the function ϕi(r)\phi_{i}(\vec{r}) to describe the position r\vec{r} in the lattice occupied by individuals of species ii, we use the mean value ϕi\langle\phi_{i}\rangle to find the Fourier transform

φi(κ)={ϕi(r)ϕi},\varphi_{i}(\vec{\kappa})=\mathcal{F}\,\{\phi_{i}(\vec{r})-\langle\phi_{i}\rangle\}, (1)

and the spectral densities

Si(k)=kx,kyφi(κ).S_{i}(\vec{k})=\sum_{k_{x},k_{y}}\,\varphi_{i}(\vec{\kappa}). (2)

The normalised inverse Fourier transform allows us to find the autocorrelation function for species ii as

Ci(r)=1{Si(k)}C(0),C_{i}(\vec{r}^{\prime})=\frac{\mathcal{F}^{-1}\{S_{i}(\vec{k})\}}{C(0)}, (3)

which we write as a function of rr as

Ci(r)=|r|=x+yCi(r)min[2N(x+y+1),(x+y+1)].C_{i}(r^{\prime})=\sum_{|\vec{r}^{\prime}|=x+y}\frac{C_{i}(\vec{r}^{\prime})}{min\left[2N-(x+y+1),(x+y+1)\right]}. (4)

Defining the threshold Ci(li)=0.15C_{i}(l_{i})=0.15, we define the characteristic length scale for spatial domains of species ii as lil_{i}, with i=1,2,3,4,5i=1,2,3,4,5.

2.7 Selection and Infection Risks

The impact of the locally adaptive organisms’ response to their survival is computed by employing two death risks: a) Selection Risk, ζi(t)\zeta_{i}(t): the probability of an organism of species ii being killed by an individual of species i1i-1 at time tt; b) Infection Risk, χi(t)\chi_{i}(t): the probability of a healthy organism of species ii being infected by an ill individual of any species at time tt.

The numerical implementation of ζi(t)\zeta_{i}(t) and χi(t)\chi_{i}(t) follows the algorithm:

  1. 1.

    We count the total number of healthy and sick individuals of species ii when each generation commences;

  2. 2.

    We calculate how many individuals of species ii are selected (killed by individuals of species i1i-1) during the generation;

  3. 3.

    We compute the number of healthy organisms infected during the generation;

  4. 4.

    The selection risk is the ratio between the number of selected individuals and the total number of organisms;

  5. 5.

    The infection risk is the ratio between the number of infected individuals and the total number of healthy organisms.

Refer to caption
(a)
Refer to caption
(b)
Refer to caption
(c)
Refer to caption
(d)
Refer to caption
(e)
Refer to caption
(f)
Refer to caption
(g)
Refer to caption
(h)
Figure 2: Spatial patterns captured from simulations in lattices with 5002500^{2} grid points of the generalised rock-paper-scissors game. Figures  2(a),  2(b),  2(c), and  2(d) show snapshots of the spatial configuration after 2020, 100100, 250250, and 20002000 generations. The colours follows the scheme in Fig. 1; empty spaces appear in white dots. Figures  2(e),  2(f),  2(g), and  2(h) highlight the organisms of species 11, with orange and purples indicating individuals performing Social Distancing and Safeguard tactics, respectively. The social distancing trigger is β=0.6\beta=0.6 and the perception radius is R=3R=3. The dynamics of the spatial patterns during the entire simulation are shown in videos https://youtu.be/tRxooHOroZo and https://youtu.be/FYH24c7A1Uc.
Refer to caption
Figure 3: Temporal variation of the densities of organisms of species ii in the realisation in Fig. 2. The grey line depicts the variation of the density of empty space, while red, blue, pink, green, and yellow stand for species 11, 22, 33, 44, and 55, respectively.

2.8 Initial conditions, parameters and figures

Throughout this paper, all outcomes presented were obtained in simulations with 𝒩=5002\mathcal{N}=500^{2} grid points, starting from random initial conditions and running for a timespan of 50005000 generations. The initial conditions are elaborated by allocating an individual of a random species at each point. The number of individuals of each species is the same at the beginning of the simulation, Ii=𝒩/5I_{i}=\mathcal{N}/5, with 1%1\% being sick - there are no empty spaces in the initial state. All simulations ran for the set of probabilities: sh=rh=mh=1/3s_{h}=r_{h}=m_{h}=1/3, ss=rs=5/31s_{s}=r_{s}=5/31, ms=9/31m_{s}=9/31, ω=10/31\omega=10/31, and c=d=1/31c=d=1/31. However, having repeated the simulations for other parameters, we verified that our conclusions are valid for other sets of interaction probabilities.

The outcomes presented in Fig. 2 and 3 were obtained for R=3R=3, λ=1.0\lambda=1.0, and β=0.6\beta=0.6. The statistical analyses whose results appear in Figs. 4 to  7 were realised by performing a series of 100100 simulations, starting from different initial conditions; in each case, the standard deviation is represented by error bars. The parameters were assumed as follows: i) Figs. 4 and  5: 0β10\leq\beta\leq 1, with intervals of Δβ=0.1\Delta\beta=0.1 (for fixed R=3R=3 and λ=1.0\lambda=1.0); ii) Fig. 6: 0R50\leq R\leq 5, with intervals of ΔR=1\Delta R=1 (for fixed β=0.6\beta=0.6 and λ=1.0\lambda=1.0); iii) Fig. 7: 0λ10\leq\lambda\leq 1, with intervals of Δλ=0.1\Delta\lambda=0.1 (for fixed R=3R=3 and β=0.6\beta=0.6).

In Figs. 2(a) to 2(d), organisms of species ii are depicted by the respective species colours, following the scheme in Fig. 1, where red, blue, pink, green, and yellow stand for species 11, 22, 33, 44, and 55, respectively; white dots show the empty sites. The organisms of species 11 present in Figs. 2(a) to 2(d) are shown in Figd. 2(e) to 2(h), with purple and orange dots indicating the individuals performing Safeguard and Social Distancing, respectively; grey areas are empty or occupied by organisms of species 22, 33, 44, 55. The colours in Figs. 3 to  7 also follow the scheme in Fig. 1; the grey lines represents empty spaces.

3 Spatial patterns

To compute the effects of the adaptive survival strategy to local epidemic surges on spatial patterns, we first ran a single simulation considering that organisms react to sick organisms’ presence in case the local density of ill individuals is higher than the 60%60\% (β=0.6\beta=0.6). Snapshots of the individuals’ spatial distribution after 2020, 100100, 250250, and 20002000 generations are shown in Figs.  2(a),  2(b),  2(c), and  2(d), respectively. White dots represent empty spaces, whereas organisms are depicted with the respective species’ colour in Fig. 1. The spatial configuration during the whole simulation is shown in the video https://youtu.be/tRxooHOroZo, whereas the temporal dynamics of the densities of species are depicted in Fig. 3.

As soon as the simulation commences, organisms of species 11 cleverly move toward the direction where the death risk is minimum, while all individuals of the other species walk randomly. The advantage of species is reflected in rapid population growth, as observed in the high magnitude in the density of species 11 in Fig. 3; the areas with red dots dominate the snapshot in Fig. 2(b) (t=100t=100 generations). The high abundance in the density of species 11 favours organisms of species 55, that take control of the areas of species 11 and grow, as shown by the yellow line in Fig. 3 (t=250t=250 generations). After the initial stage of high fluctuations in the densities of species (the pattern formation process), spiral waves spread on the lattice. The results show that the cyclic dominance symmetry inherent in the rock-paper-scissors model is broken because deaths of organisms of species 11 are rarer than fatalities that affect other species. Cleverly adapting the survival movement strategy, organisms guarantee the control of the largest fraction of territory, leading the species 11 to predominate in the cyclic game, as shown in Fig. 3.

In addition, Figs.  2(e),  2(f),  2(g), and  2(h) highlight the spatial distribution of organisms of species 11 in Figs.  2(a),  2(b),  2(c), and  2(d), respectively. When the snapshots were captured, organisms shown in orange were performing the Social Distancing tactic, while the individuals depicted by purple dots were executing the Safeguard tactic. Grey areas are occupied by individuals of species 22, 33, 44, 55, and empty spaces. Video https://youtu.be/FYH24c7A1Uc shows the dynamics of the organisms of species 11 executing each survival strategy during the entire simulation.

The results highlight the ability of organisms of species 11 to adjust the survival directional tactic in response to local disease surges. After an initial proportion of sick individuals is 1%1\%, the disease spreads through the lattice, causing local outbreaks. Because of this, the proportion of individuals performing the Social Distancing tactic (orange dots) grows significantly in the first stage of the simulations, as shown in Figs. 2(e) and 2(f). However, as each organism performs the Social Distancing strategy only if more than 60%60\% of its neighbours are infected, many of them continue executing the Safeguard strategy (purple dots). This guarantees the species to profit with the protection against selection and virus infection. Therefore, changing the directional movement responding to localised concentrations of infected organisms benefits the species whose population grows, as depicted in Fig.3.

Refer to caption
Figure 4: Characteristic length scales of the typical spatial domains occupied for species ii in terms of the social distancing trigger parameter. The results were averaged from sets of 100100 simulations; the error bars indicate the standard deviation. The colours follow the scheme in Fig. 1.
Refer to caption
(a)
Refer to caption
(b)
Refer to caption
(c)
Figure 5: Infection risk (Figure a), selection risk (Figure b), densities of species (Figure c) in terms of the social distancing trigger parameter. The outcomes were averaged from sets of 100100 simulations; the error bars show the standard deviation.

4 The role of the social distancing trigger parameter

4.1 Characteristic length scales of typical spatial domains

The spatial patterns observed in Fig. 2 show that the asymmetry in the species segregation results from the local interactions where each organism of species 11 moves strategically, optimising the results of the behavioural survival strategy according to its own reality. To quantify the characteristic length scale of the typical spatial areas occupied by each species, we ran a set of 100100 simulations for various values of β\beta. The mean value of characteristic length scale lil_{i}, with i=1,2,3,4,5i=1,2,3,4,5 is depicted in Fig. 4; the error bars represent the standard deviation.

If organisms of species 11 exclusively execute the Social Distancing tactic, independent of the perception of the local density of sick individuals (β=0.0\beta=0.0), the average size of the areas occupied by them is larger than the others. In this limit case, individuals of species 55 form the second-largest groups; the characteristic length scale of regions dominated by species 33 is the shortest. The results are approximately the same for 0.0β 0.30.0\,\leq\,\beta\,\leq\,0.3. However, as the social distancing trigger grows (individuals live with less concern about the disease contamination), the difference in the average sizes of the regions of each species decreases. For β0.8\beta\geq 0.8, organisms of species 44 are limited to the smallest areas; species 11 occupies spatial domains with the second shortest characteristic length scale.

4.2 Selection, infection risks, and densities of species

Performing a series of simulations, starting from different initial conditions, we computed the social distancing trigger parameter interferes with the organisms’ infection, selection risks, and densities of species. Figures 5(a), 5(b), and 5(c) depict ζi\zeta_{i}, χi\chi_{i}, and ρi\rho_{i} as functions of β\beta - the simulations parameters are the same as in the previous sections.

Overall, the results show a crossover between scenarios where organisms perform exclusively one out of two survival movement strategies: the Social Distancing (β0.3\beta\leq 0.3) and Safeguard (β0.9\beta\geq 0.9) tactics.

  • If the social distancing trigger is low, the organisms are more careful against disease contamination. For β 0.3\beta\,\leq\,0.3, the fear of being sick is too high that all organisms forget the risk of being killed by enemies for executing exclusively social distancing, as shown in Fig. 5(a). The consequence is that the infection risk reaches the minimum value while the selection risk is maximum for β 0.3\beta\,\leq\,0.3: the more careful the organisms are against viral infection, the more exposed to being killed by individuals of species 55.

  • Conversely, as β\beta grows, the organisms are more tolerant of the presence of viral vectors, concentrating more on approaching the enemies of their enemies. Thus, the selection risk decreases while infection risk grows for larger β\beta. According to Fig. 5(b), for β0.9\beta\geq 0.9, the individuals’ negligence in avoiding disease contamination leads to the maximum infection risk. This happens because although the protection received from organisms of species 44 against individuals of species 55 is maximum, there is a chance of part of the guards being sick, thus, infecting the individuals of species 11.

The results depicted in Fig. 5(c) show that the benefits of the adaptive survival strategy for species 11 are maximised for β=0.6\beta=0.6. In this case, species 11 predominates in the cyclic game, with the density of organisms of species 11 reaching the maximum.

5 The influence of the perception radius

To investigate the role of the organisms’ physical ability to scan the neighbourhood, we performed simulations for various values of RR considering β=0.6\beta=0.6 and λ=1.0\lambda=1.0, which brings the most significant benefit in terms of population growth for species 11, as depicted in Fig. 5(c). Figures 6(a) and 6(b) show the organisms’ infection and infection risks, respectively; the average densities of individuals of each species appear in Fig. 6(c).

We found that, for short-range perception, the signals captured provide imprecise information about the neighbourhood. Therefore, organisms neither properly detect the danger of contamination by nearby ill organisms nor accurately choose the direction to walk when performing the directional movement strategy. However, even for R=1R=1, the locally adaptive strategy is advantageous for species 11, which dominates in the cyclic game, as depicted in Fig. 6(c).

As RR increases, the environmental cues improve, giving the organisms more precise knowledge of the existence of a local epidemic outbreak: organisms’ reaction is more accurate, correctly switching the movement strategies to maximise protection against disease contamination and enemies. The results show that for R2R\geq 2, the reduction in the infection and selection risks accentuates, as depicted in Figs. 6(a) and Fig. 6(b). As the number of deaths of individuals of species 11 decreases for larger RR, the population rises, as shown in Fig. 6(c).

Refer to caption
(a)
Refer to caption
(b)
Refer to caption
(c)
Figure 6: Infection risk (Figure a), selection risk (Figure b), and densities of species (Figure c) as functions of the organisms’ perception radius. The results were averaged from 100100 simulations for β=0.6\beta=0.6 and λ=1.0\lambda=1.0; the error bars indicate the standard deviation.
Refer to caption
(a)
Refer to caption
(b)
Refer to caption
(c)
Figure 7: Infection risk (Figure a), selection risk (Figure b), and densities of species (Figure c) as functions of the organisms’ adaptation factor. The results were averaged from 100100 simulations for β=0.6\beta=0.6 and R=3R=3; the error bars indicate the standard deviation.

6 The impact of the organisms’ adaptation factor

Finally, we study how the organisms’ conditioning to interpret the environmental cues and react correctly to a local disease surge controls the benefits of the locally adaptive survival movement strategy. For this, we ran sets of 100100 simulations for many values λ\lambda, as depicted in Fig. 7(a) (infection risk), 7(b) (selection risk), and 7(c) (density of species). Following the outcomes in Fig. 5(c), we have chosen β=0.6\beta=0.6 and R=3R=3 that maximises the benefits of territorial control for species 11.

Fig. 7(a) shows that organisms of species 11 are riskier of being infected than the others. This happens because only organisms of species 11 perform the Social Distancing tactic, approaching individuals of species 44, while organisms of other species move randomly. The outcomes show that if the fraction of organisms of species 11 conditioned to perceive the presence of a local epidemic outbreak and start the Social Distancing tactic is small, χ1\chi_{1} grows instead of decreasing. This counterintuitive result holds for λ0.7\lambda\leq 0.7; however, if more than 70%70\% of individuals learn to identify local surges, the infection risk of organisms of species 11 decreases. The results show that, for λ>0\lambda>0, organisms of species 22 and 33 also benefit from dropped infection risk - although they do not perform any survival movement strategy.

Regarding the selection risk, the results depicted in Fig. 7(b) show that the risk of organisms of species 11 being killed in the cyclic game rises as λ\lambda grows. This happens because as the proportion of individuals of species 11 learning to avoid social grows, the vulnerability to being caught by an enemy increases. However, Fig. 7(c) shows that the balance is positive for species 11, which occupies the greater fraction of the grid as λ\lambda increases. The results show that the benefits for species 11 are maximised if 90%90\% of individuals are conditioned to adjust the survival movement strategy in response to local epidemic outbreaks.

Fig. 7(c) reveals that as the proportion of organisms of species 11 learning to change the survival strategy when facing a local disease surge grows, the species 55 also profits more with population growth. In the case of species 44, the spatial density increases for λ0.8\lambda\leq 0.8, with the best results obtained if λ0.4\lambda\approx 0.4. In contrast, the population decline of species 22 and 33 increase as λ\lambda grows; the density of empty remains approximately constant with the adaptation factor.

7 Discussion and Conclusions

We explored a generalised rock-paper-scissors model with a disease being transmitted between neighbours, irrespective of the species. We consider that organisms of one out of the species execute a behavioural movement strategy, where each organism moves in the direction with a higher density of individuals that can protect them against enemies. However, the alliance with enemies of enemies is not advantageous when a local surge represents an increased infection risk. In this case, if an organism is apt to recognise the existence of a local epidemic outbreak, the Safeguard strategy is avoided. Instead, the Social Distancing tactic is performed, with the individual moving in the direction with the highest density of empty spaces. In contrast with the model investigated in Ref. [27], where a fixed global design for the combination of survival strategies is assumed by any organism (independent of what is happening in its neighbourhood), here, the choice of the movement tactic is individual. Therefore, each organism is autonomous in taking the more appropriate decision according to the current circunstances, with the Social Distancing tactic substituting the Safeguard strategy only when the local epidemic outbreak reaches the organism’s neighbourhood.

The unevenness introduced by the behavioural movement strategy impacts the pattern formation, with the social distancing trigger determining the scales of the spatial regions inhabited by each species and controlling the level of protection against selection and infection. Our findings show a crossover between a regime where organisms focus only on the Social Distancing strategy (low social distancing trigger) and a scenario where they are exclusively interested in protecting themselves against death in the cyclic game (high social distancing trigger). We found that the profit in terms of population growth is maximised if both survival strategies are executed, with organisms using the Social Distancing strategy only when a local surge appears in the neighbourhood.

The organisms’ perception radius plays a central role in the local adaptation of the behavioural strategy. The results show that the further the organisms can scan the vicinity, the easier the detection of a forthcoming disease surge and more accurate is the decision of the best direction to move. This means a long-range perception radius produces a more significant reduction in the risks of individuals being killed by an enemy and contaminated by disease. The consequence is the rise of the species population for a large perception radius.

Our outcomes also show that the organisms’ adaptation factor influences population dynamics. We found that, independent of the proportion of individuals conditioned to move adaptively, the species prevails in the cyclic game. However, the fraction of territory controlled by the species increases as the adaptation factor increases.

Our simulations assume that each individual can distinguish sick from healthy neighbours; for example, symptoms of the disease are identified as a result of the infection. However, we can extend our conclusions for the case where neither all ill organisms present symptoms. As the individuals are not fully aware of the local surges, the level of precaution should be increased. Suppose that the level of symptomatic disease is η\eta, where η\eta is a real number (0η 10\,\leq\,\eta\,\leq\,1). In this case, one has β=ηβ~\beta=\eta\,\tilde{\beta}, where β~\tilde{\beta} is the minimum local density of ill organisms with symptoms triggering the Social Distancing tactic.

Acknowledgments

We thank CNPq, ECT, Fapern, and IBED for financial and technical support.

References

References

  • [1] Begon M, Townsend C R and Harper J L 2006 Ecology: from individuals to ecosystems (Oxford: Blackwell Publishing)
  • [2] Abrams P A 1984 The American Naturalist 124 80–96
  • [3] Buchholz R 2007 Trends in Ecology & Evolution 22 401 – 407 ISSN 0169-5347
  • [4] Bradshaw W E and Holzapfel C M 2006 Science 312 1477–1478
  • [5] Dukas R 2004 Annual Review of Ecology, Evolution, and Systematics 35 347–374
  • [6] Cormont A, Malinowska A H, Kostenko O, Radchuk V, Hemerik L, WallisDeVries M F and Verboom J 2011 Biodiversity and Conservation 20 483–503
  • [7] Riotte-Lambert L and Matthiopoulos J 2020 Trends in Ecology & Evolution 35 163–174
  • [8] Dittmann L and Schausberger P 2017 Scientific Reports 7 2045–2322
  • [9] Bonte D and Dahirel M Oikos 126(4)
  • [10] Benhamou S and Pierre Bovet P 1989 Animal Behaviour 38 375–383
  • [11] Bowler D E and Benton T G 2005 Biol Rev. Camb. Philos. Soc. 80 205–225
  • [12] Barraquand F and S B 2008 Ecology 89 3336–3348
  • [13] Snowden F M 2019 Epidemics and society: from the black death to the present (New Haven and London: Yale University Press)
  • [14] Pastor-Satorras R, Castellano C, Van Mieghem P and Vespignani A 2015 Rev. Mod. Phys. 87(3) 925–979
  • [15] Hu B, Guo H, Zhou P and Shi Z L 2021 Nature Reviews Microbiology 19 141
  • [16] Nagatani T, Ichinose G and ichi Tainaka K 2018 Journal of Theoretical Biology 450 66–75
  • [17] Dunn A M, Torchin M E, Hatcher M J, Kotanen P M, Blumenthal D M, Byers J E, Coon C A, Frankel V M, Holt R D, Hufbauer R A, Kanarek A R, Schierenbeck K A, Wolfe L M and Perkins S E 2012 Functional Ecology 26 1262–1274
  • [18] d’Onofrio A and Manfredi P 2022 Chaos, Solitons & Fractals 159 112072
  • [19] Pallini A, Janssen A and Sabelis M W 1997 Oecologia 110 179–185
  • [20] Reluga T C 2010 PLoS Comput. Biol. 6 e1000793
  • [21] Stockmaier S, Stroeymeyt N, Shattuck E C, Hawley D M, Meyers L A and Bolnick D I 2021 Science 371 eabc8881
  • [22] Moura B and Menezes J 2021 Scientific Reports 11 6413
  • [23] Sabelis M W, Janssen A and Kant M R 2001 Science 291 2104–2105
  • [24] Tenorio M, Rangel E and Menezes J 2022 Chaos, Solitons & Fractals 162 112430
  • [25] Menezes J, Tenorio M and Rangel E 2022 Europhysics Letters 139 57002
  • [26] Menezes J, Batista S and Rangel E 2022 Biosystems 221 104777
  • [27] Rangel E, Moura B and Menezes J 2022 Biosystems 217 104689
  • [28] Itoh Y 1973 Annals of the Institute of Statistical Mathematics 25 635–641
  • [29] Tainaka K i 1988 Journal of the Physical Society of Japan 57 2588–2590
  • [30] Szolnoki A, Mobilia M, Jiang L L, Szczesny B, Rucklidge A M and Perc M 2014 Journal of The Royal Society Interface 11 20140735
  • [31] Menezes J 2021 Phys. Rev. E 103(5) 052216
  • [32] Menezes J and Moura B 2021 Phys. Rev. E 104(5) 054201
  • [33] Menezes J, Rangel E and Moura B 2022 Ecological Informatics 69 101606
  • [34] Avelino P P, Bazeia D, Losano L, Menezes J, de Oliveira B F and Santos M A 2018 Phys. Rev. E 97(3) 032415
  • [35] Avelino P P, Bazeia D, Losano L, Menezes J and Oliveira B F 2012 Phys. Rev. E 86(3) 036112
  • [36] Menezes J and Moura B 2022 Chaos, Solitons & Fractals 157 111903
  • [37] Avelino P P, Menezes J, de Oliveira B F and Pereira T A 2019 Phys. Rev. E 99(5) 052310
  • [38] Wang W X, Lai Y C and Grebogi C 2010 Phys. Rev. E 81(4) 046113
  • [39] Nagatani T 2019 Physica A: Statistical Mechanics and its Applications 535 122531
  • [40] Yang X, Zhou H and Zhou X 2019 Science China Life Sciences 1389–1391 ISSN 1389-1391
  • [41] May R M and Leonard W J 1975 SIAM J. Appl. Math. 29 243–253