This paper was converted on www.awesomepapers.org from LaTeX by an anonymous user.
Want to know more? Visit the Converter page.

A scientific review on advances in statistical methods for crossover design

Salil Koner
Abstract

A comprehensive review of the literature on crossover design is needed to highlight its evolution, applications, and methodological advancements across various fields. Given its widespread use in clinical trials and other research domains, understanding this design’s challenges, assumptions, and innovations is essential for optimizing its implementation and ensuring accurate, unbiased results. This article extensively reviews the history and statistical inference methods for crossover designs. A primary focus is given to the AB-BA design as it is the most widely used design in literature. Extension from two periods to higher-order designs is discussed, and a general inference procedure for continuous response is studied. Analysis of multivariate and categorical responses is also reviewed in this context. A bunch of open problems in this area are shortlisted.

1 Introduction

Experimental design is essential to systematically assess the relationship between factors and responses. In a Crossover design, each experimental unit receives more than one treatment during different periods. As the treatment applied to a specific unit changes over periods, it is often called change-over or switch-over design. As per Jones & Kenward (2014), the earliest use of crossover design was in agricultural experiments around the mid-nineteenth century by Liebig et al. (1847). Since the last two decades, crossover designs have been extensively used in phase-1 or phase-2 trials of pharmaceutical studies (Lui, 2016). But, their use is not limited as it is extensively used in biological assay (Finney, 1956), for evaluation of food products by FDA (Brown Jr, 1980), weather modification experiments (Moran, 1959), psychological studies (Ruiz-Cantero et al., 2007), bio-equivalence studies (Jaki et al., 2013), consumer preference experiments (Harker et al., 2008). Senn (2002) applies such designs in pharmacokinetic studies. Several books on crossover designs, such as Jones & Kenward (2014), Ratkowsky et al. (1992), offer a detailed description of the experiments where crossover designs have been used historically.

The extensive use of crossover design stems from a major drawback of traditional parallel group design, where each subject is randomly assigned to one of two or more treatment groups. To illustrate this, assume we are interested in comparing treatments A and B. In a parallel group design, the experimental units chosen for the study are randomly assigned to one of two treatment groups, where one group receives treatment A and the other receives treatment B. To specify distinctly, no subject receives more than one treatment in a parallel group design. Let us call this design dd^{\ast}. To test for treatment contrast, we consider the traditional model,

Yik=μ+τd(i)+Eiki=1,2k=1,2,,ni\displaystyle Y_{ik}=\mu+\tau_{d^{\ast}(i)}+E_{ik}\quad\quad i=1,2\quad k=1,2,\;\dots,\;n_{i} (1)

where YikY_{ik} is the response for the kthkth subject in iith treatment group, μ\mu be the general mean, τd(i)\tau_{d^{\ast}(i)} be the (fixed) effect of treatment applied to the units in group ii under the design dd^{\ast} and EikE_{ik} be the error component. The number of experimental units in the iith group is nin_{i}, i=1,2i=1,2. This implies that under the above design dd^{\ast}, d(1)=Ad^{\ast}(1)=A and d(2)=Bd^{\ast}(2)=B. It is very natural to split the error into two components, i.e., Eik=Sik+eikE_{ik}=S_{ik}+e_{ik}, where SikS_{ik} be the (random) effect due to kkth subject in iith group and eike_{ik} be the random measurements or experimental error. Also, eike_{ik} and SikS_{ik} are independently distributed with N(0,σ2)N(0,\sigma^{2}) and N(0,σs2)N(0,\sigma_{s}^{2}) respectively. Although these two errors are not identifiable under this setup, nothing prohibits us from conceptually splitting the total random component in the above way. The idea is whether we can differentiate this random subject effect from the estimators of treatment contrast by considering some other design if the above additive split of EikE_{ik} is correct. The least squares estimator for the treatment effect τAτB\tau_{A}-\tau_{B} is Y¯1Y¯2\bar{Y}_{1}-\bar{Y}_{2} (where Y¯i\bar{Y}_{i} is the group mean i=1,2i=1,2) with its standard error (SE) (assuming n1=n2=nn_{1}=n_{2}=n) as 2(σs2+σ2)n\sqrt{\frac{2(\sigma_{s}^{2}+\sigma^{2})}{n}}. In most experimental designs, the inherent heterogeneity in the experimental units induces a high between-subject variability in the response. Then, the SE will be inflated if σs2\sigma_{s}^{2} is large compared to σ2\sigma^{2}. Naturally, an analysis of variance (ANOVA) type test for detecting a significant treatment difference has low power. On the other hand, in a crossover design where each subject receives more than one treatment in different periods, it is possible to estimate every treatment contrast (and possibly contrasts of all other effects) through within-subject difference after eliminating variability due to the subject. If the magnitude of within-subject variation σ2\sigma^{2} is minimal, then the test can be done with high power. It is reasonable to contemplate that there can be a confounding effect of the period on the response, but this can be eliminated by splitting the units into two groups where one group receives A in period 1 and B in period 2. Another receives B in period 1 and A in period 2. Such a design, named as 2×2×22\times 2\times 2 design or AB-BA design, is the most commonly used crossover design in the literature, compared to the other complex crossover designs where the objective is to compare more than two treatments and/or responses are observed for each subject over more than two periods. Also, when we have more than two treatments, the models for the carryover effect become more complex. We will discuss those in more detail in Section 4. Moreover, crossover designs are generally more economical as fewer subjects are required to get the same number of responses compared to parallel-group studies.

One potential objection to this crossover design is that the effect of treatment in period 2 is confounded by the residual effect of the treatments applied in period 1. This residual effect is termed as carryover effect of treatment (Cochran et al., 1941). In most trials, the objective is to measure the direct impact of the treatment on the response after adjusting for carryover. Efficient estimation of direct treatment effect through the within-subject difference in the presence of significant carryover can be done by choosing a suitable crossover design with more than two periods and/or more than two groups. However, carryover hinders a robust estimation of the direct treatment effect in an AB-BA design. Much research has been done in the late twentieth century to address this problem. We will discuss those in Section 3. The use of crossover design is limited to applications where the treatment might change the initial condition of the experimental unit significantly. For example, if a surgery completely cures a patient, the patient’s condition before and after surgery is not the same. With that perspective, crossover designs are particularly effective in assessing treatments that can’t cure the subject entirely but moderate the disease, such as migraine or asthma (Senn, 2002).

The rest of the paper is organized as follows. Section 2 consolidates the list of notations we will use throughout the rest of the paper. In Section 3, we study the existing inference procedures for AB-BA design when the response is continuous. Further, we discuss the advantages of higher order designs in Section 4 and review some inference procedures. In Section 5, we revisit AB-BA design to explore the use of baseline measurements. Section 6 includes the inference techniques for a multivariate response. Statistical methods for analyzing binary or categorical responses have been discussed in Section 7. Concluding remarks and possible areas of further research are summarized in Section 8.

2 Notation

We will stick to the following notation and terminology for the rest of the paper. Three parameters, viz, define a crossover design. tt : number of treatments to be compared, gg : number of treatment groups, pp : number of periods over which responses are observed. Throughout the paper, we denote a crossover design with these parameters as d(t,p,g)d(t,p,g). It is crucial to notice that, unlike parallel group design, the number of treatment groups (gg) is not always equal to the number of treatments to be compared (tt). Here, a treatment group is defined by a sequence of treatments, and the sequence length is the number of periods in the study. The experimental units selected for the study are randomly assigned to one of the gg treatment groups. We define nin_{i} as the number of units in the iith group, i=1,2,,gi=1,2,\;\dots,\;g. n1+n2++ng=Nn_{1}+n_{2}+\dots+n_{g}=N, the total number of subjects or units in the study.

Consider a small example with t=2t=2, p=3p=3 and g=4g=4. There are two treatments to be compared (say A and B), each subject is assigned to one of the four treatment groups, and the response is observed over three periods. An example of a d(2,3,4)d(2,3,4) design is presented in table 1(a). Each row defines a treatment group, and the columns represent the period. The four treatment groups are ABA, BAB, AAB, and BBA. Each cell represents the treatment applied to a subject in a treatment group and a particular period. For example, a subject in treatment group 33 receives treatment A at period 11, again A in period 22 and B in period 33. This implies that each subject gets multiple treatments throughout the study. In other words, the treatments are switched over across the periods; this is why it is called a crossover or switch-over design. Similarly, table 1(b) provides a diagram of another crossover design with three treatments (A, B, and C), four periods, and six treatment groups, i.e., a d(3,4,6)d(3,4,6) design. To clarify, a subject in group 55 receives treatment B in period 11, A in period 22, and treatment C in periods 33 and 44. Subjects in groups 33 start with treatment C, followed by A, B, and B again over the four study periods.

Group Period
1 2 3
1 A B A
2 B A B
3 A A B
4 B B A
(a) Diagram of a d(2,3,4) design
Group Period
1 2 3 4
1 A B C C
2 B C A A
3 C A B B
4 A C B B
5 B A C C
6 C B A A
(b) Diagram of a d(3,4,6) design
Table 1: A visual representation of crossover design with two (in (a)) and three (in (b)) treatments, four periods, and six treatment groups

YijkY_{ijk} is the single variate response for kkth subject in iith sequence at period jj. Except for Section 7, the response is assumed to be continuous. The parameter μ\mu is termed as the overall/general mean, πj\pi_{j} as the effect of jjth period, τd(i,j)\tau_{d(i,j)} as the direct effect of treatment administered to the iith group at period jj and λd(i,j1)\lambda_{d(i,j-1)} as the carryover effect of treatment at period j1j-1 for iith group. For example, under the design d(3,4,6)d(3,4,6) in table 1(b), d(2,3)=Ad(2,3)=A, d(4,2)=Cd(4,2)=C, d(5,1)=Bd(5,1)=B etc. It is assumed that there is no carryover effect in the first period, i.e., λd(i,0)=0\lambda_{d(i,0)}=0. SikS_{ik} be the effect of kkth subject in group ii. Unless otherwise specified, SikS_{ik} is assumed to be random. Lastly, eijke_{ijk} is the random error of YijkY_{ijk}. We refer the subscript ii as the sequence, jj as the period, kk as the subject (i=1g,j=1p,k=1(1)nii=1\dots g,j=1\dots p,k=1(1)n_{i} and i=1gni=N\sum_{i=1}^{g}n_{i}=N). Now, we will define certain terms that are general to any crossover design but are mainly introduced for AB-BA design.

Tik=Yi1k+Yi2k;Pik=Yi1kYi2k;Cik={Yi1kYi2kif i=1( group AB)Yi2kYi1kif i=2( group BA)\displaystyle T_{ik}=Y_{i1k}+Y_{i2k}\;\;;\quad P_{ik}=Y_{i1k}-Y_{i2k}\;\;;\quad C_{ik}=\begin{cases}Y_{i1k}-Y_{i2k}\;&\textrm{if }i=1(\textrm{ group AB})\\ Y_{i2k}-Y_{i1k}\;&\textrm{if }i=2(\textrm{ group BA})\end{cases} (2)

TikT_{ik}, PikP_{ik}, CikC_{ik} are called subject totals, period difference, and crossover difference, respectively. T¯i\bar{T}_{i\bullet}, P¯i\bar{P}_{i\bullet}, C¯i\bar{C}_{i\bullet} be average of them over kk. An M×1M\times 1 multivariate response recorded for kkth subject of iith group in jjth period is denoted by 𝐘ijk{\bm{\mathbf{{Y}}}}_{ijk}.

3 Inference procedures for continuous response under AB-BA design

This section discusses existing methodologies for analyzing continuous response under AB-BA design. The key point is that the response of a subject in group AB at period 2 will depend not only on the effect of treatment B but also on the residual effect of A that is applied in period 1 and similarly for group BA. A traditional model that adjusts for the carryover effect follows.

Yijk=μ+πj+τd(i,j)+λd(i,j1)+Sik+eijk\displaystyle Y_{ijk}=\mu+\pi_{j}+\tau_{d(i,j)}+\lambda_{d(i,j-1)}+S_{ik}+e_{ijk} (3)

We assume Sik,eijkS_{ik},e_{ijk} are independently distributed with mean 0, variance σs2\sigma_{s}^{2} and σ2\sigma^{2} respectively. Under model (3), the expected value of YijkY_{ijk} for four different cells are presented in table 2.

Sequence Period-1 Period-2 Period Diff Period Total
E(Y1jk)E(Y_{1jk}) μ+π1+τA\mu+\pi_{1}+\tau_{A} μ+π2+τB+λA\mu+\pi_{2}+\tau_{B}+\lambda_{A} π+τλA\pi+\tau-\lambda_{A} 2μ+π1+π2+τA+τB+λA2\mu+\pi_{1}+\pi_{2}+\tau_{A}+\tau_{B}+\lambda_{A}
E(Y2jk)E(Y_{2jk}) μ+π1+τB\mu+\pi_{1}+\tau_{B} μ+π2+τA+λB\mu+\pi_{2}+\tau_{A}+\lambda_{B} πτλB\pi-\tau-\lambda_{B} 2μ+π1+π2+τA+τB+λB2\mu+\pi_{1}+\pi_{2}+\tau_{A}+\tau_{B}+\lambda_{B}
Diff τ\tau λ\lambda - τ\tau 2τλ2\tau-\lambda λ\lambda
Table 2: Expected response for different cells in an AB-BA crossover design

The parameter of interests π=π1π2\pi=\pi_{1}-\pi_{2}, τ=τAτB\tau=\tau_{A}-\tau_{B}, λ=λAλB\lambda=\lambda_{A}-\lambda_{B} be the contrast of period, direct treatment and carryover effects respectively. Also, as an identifiability condition, we set λA+λB=0=τA+τB=π1+π2\lambda_{A}+\lambda_{B}=0=\tau_{A}+\tau_{B}=\pi_{1}+\pi_{2}. From the table, it is clear that we can’t construct an unbiased estimator for the carryover effect based on within-subject differences. Moreover, under Gaussian assumption, assuming n1=n2=nn_{1}=n_{2}=n

Tτ,W=(P¯1P¯2)/2N(τλ/2,σ2/n)andTτ,B=(Y¯11Y¯21)N(τ, 2(σs2+σ2)/n)\displaystyle T_{\tau,W}=\left(\bar{P}_{1\bullet}-\bar{P}_{2\bullet}\right)/2\sim N\left(\tau-\lambda/2,\;\sigma^{2}/n\right)\;\;\;\textrm{and}\quad\;\;T_{\tau,B}=\left(\bar{Y}_{11\bullet}-\bar{Y}_{21\bullet}\right)\sim N\left(\tau,\;2(\sigma_{s}^{2}+\sigma^{2})/n\right) (4)
Tπ,W=(C¯1C¯2)/2N(πλ/2,σ2/n)andTλ=T¯1T¯2N(λ, 4(2σs2+σ2)/n)\displaystyle T_{\pi,W}=\left(\bar{C}_{1\bullet}-\bar{C}_{2\bullet}\right)/2\sim N\left(\pi-\lambda/2,\;\sigma^{2}/n\right)\;\;\;\textrm{and}\quad\;\;T_{\lambda}=\bar{T}_{1\bullet}-\bar{T}_{2\bullet}\sim N\left(\lambda,\;4(2\sigma^{2}_{s}+\sigma^{2})/n\right) (5)

The estimator Tτ,WT_{\tau,W} in (4) based on within-subject difference is unbiased for treatment effect only if there is no carry-over effect. So, we can do a student-t or F test using this only if λ=0\lambda=0. An unbiased estimator Tτ,BT_{\tau,B} for the treatment effect (τ\tau) based on only first-period responses as in (5) can be constructed. However, that has a higher standard error due to between-subject variability. The criticism is that if we estimate τ\tau based on only one period, then there is no essence of doing a crossover study. However, this is exclusively a problem for AB-BA crossover design. We will see in the next section that this problem can be alleviated by studying for more than two periods. Many researchers recommended incorporating washout periods between the two periods of the study to nullify the effect of carryover (Senn, 1996). However, including a washout period is not always feasible because it will elongate the duration of the study or due to ethical reasons (Bose & Dey, 2009). Another issue of the AB-BA design is a treatment-by-period interaction and a carryover effect that might affect the response. In a 2×2×22\times 2\times 2 crossover study, the carryover effect is aliased with this interaction effect (Armitage & Hills, 1982). One can understand this by noting that there are only 4 cell means, which means a total of 3 degrees of freedom (d.f) can be accounted for, out of which 2 of them are attributed to treatment and period effect. So, only one d.f is left to identify any other effect. Still, many researchers have worked on developing methodologies for testing treatment and carryover effects in an AB-BA crossover design over the past few decades because 1) it is very easy to implement the randomization of subjects to groups and treatments compared to more complicated design, 2) If a design lasts longer than two periods, then subject drop-out can pose difficulty in the analysis (Sheehe, 1961). 3) The cost of recruiting subjects increases as the number of periods increases.

3.1 Parametric approach for testing fixed effects

Cox (1958) analyzed the crossover design, assuming that there is no carryover effect, and pointed out that it is nothing but a split-plot design. Grizzle (1965) pioneered testing for treatment effect in the presence of carryover assuming model (3) and calculated the analysis of variance (ANOVA) table. A correct table version can be found in the appendix of Hills & Armitage (1979). The author concluded that if there is a significant carryover, it is better to use a randomized parallel-group trial than a crossover design. But, when the presence of residual is in doubt, he proposed a two-stage (TS) procedure that first tests for carryover effect based on TλT_{\lambda} in (5) and then tests for treatment effect using the statistic Tτ,WT_{\tau,W} if the test for carryover is not significant, otherwise tests using Tτ,BT_{\tau,B}. It is worth noting that the preliminary carryover test lacks power as the standard error of the statistic TλT_{\lambda} is influenced by between-subject variation. Hence, the author recommends testing at a higher significance level (10%10\%). Under the normality assumption, the test-statistics for testing H0:τ=0H_{0}:\tau=0 is given by

Fco=nTτ,w2/σ^2F1,2n2(ϕ((τλ/2)2))\displaystyle F_{co}=nT_{\tau,w}^{2}/\hat{\sigma}^{2}\sim F_{1,2n-2}\left(\phi\left(\left(\tau-\lambda/2\right)^{2}\right)\right) (6)
Fpar=nTτ,b2/2[σ^2+σ^s2]F1,2n2(ϕ(τ2))\displaystyle F_{par}=nT_{\tau,b}^{2}/2\left[\hat{\sigma}^{2}+\hat{\sigma}^{2}_{s}\right]\sim F_{1,2n-2}\left(\phi\left(\tau^{2}\right)\right) (7)
FTS=FcoI(|Tλ|c)+FparI(|Tλ|>c)\displaystyle F_{TS}=F_{co}\;I\left(\lvert T_{\lambda}\rvert\leq c\right)+F_{par}\;I\left(\lvert T_{\lambda}\rvert>c\right) (8)

Where cc is an appropriate cutoff value and σ^,σs^\hat{\sigma},\hat{\sigma_{s}} be the maximum likelihood estimators of σ,σs\sigma,\sigma_{s} respectively. The subscripts of the F-statistics in (6)-(8) are meant to highlight that the tests are based on crossover (co) and parallel group (par) trials and two-stage procedure (TS). The non-centrality parameters vanish when τλ/2=0\tau-\lambda/2=0 and τ=0\tau=0, respectively. Confidence intervals (CI) for the treatment effect can be obtained by inverting the above FF tests. This can be done using SAS procedure proc mixed (SAS Institute Inc, 2008) or glm() function in R (R Core Team, 2018)

Brown Jr (1980) analyzed the cost efficiency of the crossover design by incorporating the sample size that would be required to get the same precision and concluded that if there is insignificant carryover, crossover design is super economical than a parallel group design, but then it can be costlier otherwise.

Under the null hypothesis τ=0\tau=0, FparF_{par} has a central F-distribution. If λ0\lambda\neq 0, the non-centrality parameter in FcoF_{co} does not vanish, and the test becomes biased. Willan & Pater (1986) pointed out that if there is no carryover effect without the treatment effect, then under H0H_{0} FcoF_{co} has a central F-distribution. Consequently, the test becomes unbiased. But, the problem occurs otherwise. They mentioned that for a placebo-controlled trial, or when a new treatment is compared against a standard treatment, it is expected not to have a carryover when there are no treatment effects. They also differentiated between physical and psychological carryover and asserted that significant carryover can exist without treatment effect for the former but not for the latter. The authors compared the power of the crossover and parallel group tests and derived

Pco(τ,λ)>Ppar(τ,λ)if|λ/τ|<22(1ρ)\displaystyle P_{co}(\tau,\lambda)>P_{par}(\tau,\lambda)\quad\textrm{if}\quad\left\lvert\lambda/\tau\right\rvert<2-\sqrt{2(1-\rho)} (9)

where PP is the power function of the crossover and parallel group tests and ρ=σs2/(σs2+σ2)\rho=\sigma_{s}^{2}/(\sigma_{s}^{2}+\sigma^{2}) is the intra-group correlation. This means the power of F-tests in (6) and (7) depends on the value of intra-group correlation and the relative magnitude of carryover and treatment effect. Given σs2>σ2\sigma_{s}^{2}>\sigma^{2}, if the carryover effect is small relative to the direct treatment effect, the crossover test of τ\tau can outperform the parallel group test. Later on, Willan (1988) proposed to use the maximum test-statistic Fmax=max(Fco,Fpar)F_{max}=\max\;(F_{co},F_{par}) for testing the treatment effect under the assumption that no differential carryover exists in the absence of treatment effects.

Because the test-statistic TλT_{\lambda} and Tτ,BT_{\tau,B} are highly correlated, the TS procedure can be biased even if λ\lambda is small. Freeman (1989) examined the performance of FcoF_{co}, FparF_{par} and FTSF_{TS} for testing treatment effect but they allowed the possibility that λ\lambda can be significant even if τ=0\tau=0. Simulation results showed that the nominal significance level of the TS procedure is higher than the stipulated type-I error (α\alpha) even when λ=0\lambda=0; it goes far above the actual as λ\lambda deviates from zero. In the absence of carryover, FTSF_{TS} and FcoF_{co} are more powerful than FparF_{par}, but FTSF_{TS} achieves superiority at the expense of increased significance level. Senn (1996) proposed a correction to the TS procedure to maintain the type-I error rate (α\alpha). If the type-I error of the carryover test is α1\alpha_{1}, he proposed to conduct the FparF_{par} test at a reduced significance level of αα1\alpha\alpha_{1}. This procedure is conservative except when within-subject variability σ2=0\sigma^{2}=0. Wang & Hung (1997) proposes another two-stage procedure that controls the type-error rate like the above and offers a detailed comparison between maximum test statistics and two-stage procedures. The conclusion is similar: if the relationship between carryover and treatment is unknown, then the two-stage procedures can be very misleading. But, under the assumption that carryover does not persist in the absence of treatment effect, λ/τ\lambda/\tau is negative and large in magnitude, two-stage procedures are more robust to the possible overestimation that maximum test-statistic or the crossover test can lead to.

Another disturbing feature of the TS procedure mentioned by (Senn (2002)) is if there is a significant effect of sequence (which is aliased with the carryover in AB/BA design), the estimator Tτ,bT_{\tau,b} is not unbiased for τ\tau. The test of treatment effect based on only first-period observations will be biased.

3.2 Bayesian approach

The performances of all the existing frequentist approaches rely on the information on the significance of carryover. Selwyn et al. (1981) pioneered the Bayesian analysis for the crossover trials in the Bio-equivalence study by incorporating the uncertainty as an informative prior to the carryover effect. Arguing that such information is rarely available in practice, Grieve (1985) assumed a completely uninformative prior for the parameters. The posterior distribution of τ\tau is centered at the difference of the first-period observation, and the second-period observation comes through the variance. This means if we do not assume that the carryover effect is insignificant, then we have to do our analysis based on first-period observations, a similar point made by Grizzle (1965). To incorporate a prior belief on the carryover effect, Grieve (1985) proposed a Bayes factor approach that quantifies the model’s probability with carryover and no carryover. This is more practical than Selwyn et al. (1981) approach because the variance σR2\sigma_{R}^{2} in the prior distribution of λ\lambda is not available in practice. He mentioned that if we have prior knowledge about carryover, then we should have prior information on the treatment effect too, so why not incorporate that information in the prior of τ\tau? The conclusion is that the inference for the treatment effect is heavily based on our prior belief on the carryover effect, which all the frequentist approaches conclude.

Often, the experimenter does not have an adequate extent of certainty about the carryover effects. Therefore, a complete specification of the prior distribution becomes difficult. David & Seaman Jr (2002) introduced a hierarchical approach for the Bayesian analysis of two-period crossover design to ensure robustness to prior choice. Li & Sivaganesan (2017) proposed an objective Bayesian estimation approach for AB/BA crossover trials. The authors assume a mixture distribution of the model with carryover and no carryover similar to Grieve (1985), in addition to a Zellner–Snow prior distribution (Zellner & Siow, 1980) for λ\lambda when carryover is present. The simulation study shows that the treatment effect’s nominal coverage probability is maintained close to 95%95\% level, irrespective of whether λ\lambda is small or large. In terms of MSE, this method significantly improves the TS procedure when λ\lambda is moderate.

3.3 Non-parametric approach

Contemporary to Grizzle (1965)’s parametric inference, Koch (1972) proposed a non-parametric approach for analyzing 2×2×22\times 2\times 2 crossover design using Wilcoxon rank sum test or equivalently by Mann-Whitney test. Let Rik,R~ikR_{ik},\tilde{R}_{ik} be respectively the rank of subject totals TikT_{ik} and period differences PikP_{ik} for the kth subject in the ith group based on the entire sample. Let, Ri=kRikR_{i}=\sum_{k}R_{ik} and R~i=kR~ik,i=1,2\tilde{R}_{i}=\sum_{k}\tilde{R}_{ik},i=1,2. The Wilcoxon rank-sum test-statistic for testing H01:λ=0H_{01}:\lambda=0 and H02:τ=0H_{02}:\tau=0 under λ=0\lambda=0, is given by,

Zλ=R1E(R1)Var(R1);Zτ=R~1E(R~1)Var(R~1)\displaystyle Z_{\lambda}=\frac{R_{1}-E(R_{1})}{\sqrt{\textrm{Var}(R_{1})}}\quad;\quad Z_{\tau}=\frac{\tilde{R}_{1}-E(\tilde{R}_{1})}{\sqrt{\textrm{Var}(\tilde{R}_{1})}} (10)

Where the expectation and variance of R1R_{1} and R~1\tilde{R}_{1} under the null hypothesis can be obtained from Mann & Whitney (1947). If the sample size is large, under null, the test statistics follow an approximately standard normal distribution. The exact CI for τ\tau can be obtained using the methods of Hodges Jr & Lehmann (1963) by computing the median of all possible response differences in the two groups. SAS procedure proc nparway and R function wilcox.test() offers CI, an exact and asymptotic significance test. It should be noted that the test of treatment effect is valid only if λ=0\lambda=0. Otherwise, the test should be done based on period-1 responses. A similar two-stage procedure can also be adopted here to test for the effects of the treatment. Koch (1972) also proposed non-parametric test for testing H03:λ=0,τ=0H_{03}:\lambda=0,\tau=0. Under H03H_{03}, the responses for each subject are an identically distributed bivariate random variable, and the Wilcoxon rank-sum test of location following Chatterjee & Sen (1964) can be constructed. Let γijk\gamma_{ijk} be the rank of the kth subject in group ii on period jj based on the entire sample. Define 𝜸ik=(γi1k,γi2k){\bm{\mathbf{{\gamma}}}}_{ik}=(\gamma_{i1k},\gamma_{i2k})^{\top} as the rank vector and 𝜸¯i=(γ¯i1,γ¯i2)\bar{{\bm{\mathbf{{\gamma}}}}}_{i}=(\bar{\gamma}_{i1},\bar{\gamma}_{i2})^{\top}, where γ¯ij=k=1niγijk/nii=1,2\bar{\gamma}_{ij}=\sum_{k=1}^{n_{i}}\gamma_{ijk}/n_{i}\;i=1,2 and 𝜸¯=(n1𝜸¯1+n2𝜸¯2)/(n1+n2)\bar{{\bm{\mathbf{{\gamma}}}}}=\left(n_{1}\bar{{\bm{\mathbf{{\gamma}}}}}_{1}+n_{2}\bar{{\bm{\mathbf{{\gamma}}}}}_{2}\right)/(n_{1}+n_{2}) Then, the test statistic is given by

Wτ=(𝜸¯1𝜸¯2)𝚺γ1(𝜸¯1𝜸¯2)χ22(under H03 asymptotically)\displaystyle W_{\tau}=\left(\bar{{\bm{\mathbf{{\gamma}}}}}_{1}-\bar{{\bm{\mathbf{{\gamma}}}}}_{2}\right)^{\top}{\bm{\mathbf{{\Sigma}}}}_{\gamma}^{-1}\left(\bar{{\bm{\mathbf{{\gamma}}}}}_{1}-\bar{{\bm{\mathbf{{\gamma}}}}}_{2}\right)\sim\chi^{2}_{2}\quad(\textrm{under }H_{03}\textrm{ asymptotically}) (11)
where 𝚺γ=(n1+n2)n1n2(n1+n21)i=12k=1ni(𝜸ik𝜸¯)(𝜸ik𝜸¯)\displaystyle{\bm{\mathbf{{\Sigma}}}}_{\gamma}=\frac{(n_{1}+n_{2})}{n_{1}n_{2}(n_{1}+n_{2}-1)}\sum_{i=1}^{2}\sum_{k=1}^{n_{i}}\left({\bm{\mathbf{{\gamma}}}}_{ik}-\bar{{\bm{\mathbf{{\gamma}}}}}\right)\left({\bm{\mathbf{{\gamma}}}}_{ik}-\bar{{\bm{\mathbf{{\gamma}}}}}\right)^{\top} (12)

The non-parametric tests described in (11) are based on the assumption that the variations of the samples are the same for both groups. Cornell (1991) proposed two non-parametric tests for equality of the dispersion of the responses in two groups. One is based on the rank correlation coefficient of TikT_{ik} and VikV_{ik}, and the other is based on their concordance. The test is based on the fact that under model (3), Cov(Tik,Pik)=σ12σ22,i=1,2\textrm{Cov}(T_{ik},P_{ik})=\sigma_{1}^{2}-\sigma_{2}^{2},i=1,2, which vanishes under the null hypothesis.

Tudor & Koch (1994) proposed a more general non-parametric approach to test for significant total treatment (combined of direct and carryover) effect. To accomplish that, they tested H0:2τλ=0H_{0}:2\tau-\lambda=0. The test statistics are based on the period differences of two groups Pik,i=1,2P_{ik},i=1,2, which is given by

Q=(P¯1P¯2)2Sp2;where Sp2=(n1+n2)n1n2(n1+n21)i=12k=1ni(PikP¯)2\displaystyle Q=\frac{\left(\bar{P}_{1\bullet}-\bar{P}_{2\bullet}\right)^{2}}{S_{p}^{2}}\quad;\quad\textrm{where }S_{p}^{2}=\frac{(n_{1}+n_{2})}{n_{1}n_{2}(n_{1}+n_{2}-1)}\sum_{i=1}^{2}\sum_{k=1}^{n_{i}}(P_{ik}-\bar{P})^{2} (13)

Where P¯=(n1P¯1+n2P¯2)/(n1+n2)\bar{P}=(n_{1}\bar{P}_{1\bullet}+n_{2}\bar{P}_{2\bullet})/(n_{1}+n_{2}). Under H0H_{0}, if nin_{i} are large (>15>15), QQ follows approximately χ2\chi^{2} distribution with 11 degrees of freedom. The authors mentioned the above sample size requirement for each group to ensure that the total number of subjects in the study is large enough (>30>30) to hold asymptotic results. As they suggested, the above test has a higher power of detecting any significant total treatment effect because it is entirely based on within-subject differences. If the test is rejected, further tests must be done to detect whether the difference is due to direct treatment and/or carryover effects. In the second stage, a test for carryover based on ZλZ_{\lambda} needs to be done. If the test is rejected, a test for τ\tau based on first-period responses can be done; otherwise, a test using ZτZ_{\tau} as in (10) can be carried out. Sometimes, knowledge of the length of the washout period can be incorporated to judge the absence of carryover effects. This involves a three-stage procedure. The authors recommended setting the significance levels for each test before the start of the test to ensure that the nominal significance level is not inflated.

Jung & Koch (1999) proposed a rank measure association-based non-parametric test under crossover design applicable to an ordinal response. Consider a randomized parallel group design with two treatments. Let μi\mu_{i} be the mean response for the ith group. The difference μ1μ2\mu_{1}-\mu_{2} represents a measure of disparity between the two groups. In the parametric approach, we model this disparity as μ1μ2=E(X1kX2k)=τAτB\mu_{1}-\mu_{2}=E(X_{1k}-X_{2k})=\tau_{A}-\tau_{B}. For the Wilcoxon rank-sum test, we model the median of differences between the responses through τAτB\tau_{A}-\tau_{B}. Similarly, Jung proposed to model the disparity between the two groups ψ=P(X1kX2k>0)\psi=P(X_{1k}-X_{2k}>0) non-parametrically by τ=τAτB\tau=\tau_{A}-\tau_{B}. In an AB-BA crossover design, we have 44 cell means. So, there will be four different rank measures, 𝝍=(ψ1,ψ2,ψ3,ψ4){\bm{\mathbf{{\psi}}}}=(\psi_{1},\psi_{2},\psi_{3},\psi_{4})^{\top}.

If 𝝍^\hat{{\bm{\mathbf{{\psi}}}}} be the estimate of 𝝍=(ψ1,ψ2,ψ3,ψ4){\bm{\mathbf{{\psi}}}}=(\psi_{1},\psi_{2},\psi_{3},\psi_{4})^{\top} and 𝜷=(τ,π,λ){\bm{\mathbf{{\beta}}}}=(\tau,\pi,\lambda)^{\top} be our parameter of interest, then, they proposed the model 𝐟^=logit(𝝍^)=𝐗𝜷\hat{{\bm{\mathbf{{f}}}}}=\textrm{logit}(\hat{{\bm{\mathbf{{\psi}}}}})={\bm{\mathbf{{X}}}}{\bm{\mathbf{{\beta}}}} where the design matrix 𝐗{\bm{\mathbf{{X}}}} can easily be obtained from the relation between 𝝍{\bm{\mathbf{{\psi}}}} and 𝜷{\bm{\mathbf{{\beta}}}}. The estimates 𝝍^m\hat{{\bm{\mathbf{{\psi}}}}}_{m} is constructed non-parametrically using the U-statistics of the responses related in mmth comparison m=14m=1\dots 4, e.g., for the first comparison, we consider only the responses in the first period. The author also derived the asymptotic multivariate normal distribution of the estimator 𝜷^\hat{{\bm{\mathbf{{\beta}}}}} with a suitably chosen covariance matrix, which can be used to test any hypothesis of the form H0:𝐂𝜷=0H_{0}:{\bm{\mathbf{{C}}}}{\bm{\mathbf{{\beta}}}}=0 and to derive approximate 100(1α)%100(1-\alpha)\% CI. The performance of this method is not shown in the paper through simulation but is applied to real data, and the results were similar to the standard procedures. For an extensive application of the method, see Chapter 22 of Jones & Kenward (2014).

4 Higher order crossover designs

Non-estimability of the carryover effect under model (3) is solely a problem of AB-BA crossover design and undermines its effectiveness. Complex models can be analyzed with higher power using higher-order crossover designs. By higher order, we mean either the design consists of more than two periods and/or the subjects are randomly assigned to more than two groups. For example, instead of having two groups, we can have four treatment groups: AB, BA, AA, and BB. Sometimes, it is impractical to stretch the study to more than two periods. In those situations, randomizing subjects into more than two sequence groups is essential for efficient estimation of treatment effects. Any design can be specified by three parameters: number of treatments (tt), number of periods (pp), and number of groups (gg). The tests for treatment and carryover effects gain power because each effect in the model (3) can be estimated using within-subject difference. Moreover, the carryover, treatment by period interaction, and group effects are identifiable and can be estimated separately. The estimability of effects in any crossover design can be understood in the following way. Consider a design d(t,p,g)d(t,p,g), with pgpg cell means, which means there are pg1pg-1 degrees of freedom, out of that g1g-1 attributed to group effects, p1p-1 is for period effects, the remaining (p1)(g1)(p-1)(g-1) d.f is split into treatment effect, treatment by period interaction, carryover effect.

4.1 Complex carryover models

For higher order designs with more than two periods, the model in (3) assumes that the carryover effect of a treatment in the period jj depends only on the treatment in period j1j-1. This is called first-order carryover. Moreover, the carryover effect of a treatment on itself (self-carryover) is assumed to be identical to the carryover of the treatment on another treatment (mixed-carryover), e.g. in the design d(2,3,4)d(2,3,4) shown in table 1(a), although the second group receives treatment A and the group 44 receives the same treatment (B) again at period 22, carryover effect of B from the first period in group 22 assumed to be the same as the carryover of B in group 44. However, in many practical applications, this simple carryover model is inadequate. For example, in customer survey experiments for the evaluation of certain products, if a person strongly likes or dislikes a product, its lingering effect influences the evaluation of the products that follow it. If the product is intensely disliked, the subsequent product will likely get a below-average evaluation. Also, this lingering effect diminishes proportionally as the person evaluates other products. Similarly, in pharmaceutical trials, while comparing more than two drugs under a crossover design, the carryover effect of a drug in a certain period can be significantly different if the same drug is administered in the next period compared to the carryover effect if a different drug is applied in the next period. This is because the impact of a drug is significantly dependent on the combination in which the other drugs are administered. Senn (2002) provides a mathematical explanation to justify that this simple carryover model is implausible in the pharmacokinetic study (see Chapter 1010 for details).

These problems can be addressed by including an interaction between treatment and carryover effects (Kershner & Federer, 1981). However, including an interaction term is feasible for a two-treatment design (d.f. for the interaction is 1), but for a study with several treatments, many interaction terms will be present in the model. To tackle this difficulty, Afsarinejad & Hedayat (2002) introduced a model that differentiates self-carryover and mixed carryover effects. Kempton et al. (2001) introduced another model where the carryover effect of a treatment in the current period is proportional to the treatment effect in the previous period. Another approach is to include a second-order carryover term in the model. That means the response at period jj will also be affected by the treatment at period j2,j3j-2,j\geq 3. An excellent repository of models with different carryover effects can be found in Ozan & Stufken (2010). We assume that all the parameters are estimable through within-subject differences. A general model is

Yijk=μ+πj+αi+βd(i,j)+Sik+𝜸𝐁ik+ϵijk\displaystyle Y_{ijk}=\mu+\pi_{j}+\alpha_{i}+\beta_{d(i,j)}+S_{ik}+{\bm{\mathbf{{\gamma}}}}^{\top}{\bm{\mathbf{{B}}}}_{ik}+\epsilon_{ijk} (14)

where αi\alpha_{i} is the effect of ith group, βd(i,j)\beta_{d(i,j)} is the effect of the treatment and carryover associated with jjth period for iith group and 𝐁ik{\bm{\mathbf{{B}}}}_{ik} be the vector of baseline covariates measured for subject kk in group ii. Any baseline covariates could also be included in the basic model (3), defined for AB-BA design. Depending on the design, one can include treatment by period interaction term and other factors in the model. Different forms of βd(i,j)\beta_{d(i,j)} are summarized in chapter 4 of Jones & Kenward (2014).

4.2 Optimal designs

In a crossover study with tt treatments, we aim to efficiently estimate any pair of treatment contrast τiτj\tau_{i}-\tau_{j}. To accomplish that, we want to design the study in such a way that the SE of the estimator, Var(τ^iτ^j)\textrm{Var}(\hat{\tau}_{i}-\hat{\tau}_{j}) is minimized compared to that for any competing design. Kiefer (1975) introduced different optimality criteria based on the total variance of the estimator of all possible treatment contrasts. This is essentially a function of the trace of information matrix CdC_{d} under the model assumed. Similarly, optimality criteria for an estimation of the carryover effect can also be defined. Hedayat et al. (1978) pioneered finding optimal designs by considering a subclass of design called uniform designs. In a uniform design, each treatment is assigned to the same number of subjects in each period, and for each subject, every treatment is applied in the same number of periods. Uniformity is appealing because the treatment and carryover effect becomes orthogonal to each other, and it implies p=λ1tp=\lambda_{1}t and g=λ2tg=\lambda_{2}t, where λ1,λ2\lambda_{1},\lambda_{2} is a positive integer. However, assuming p=tp=t and g=λ2tg=\lambda_{2}t, they proved that balanced uniform (BU) designs are universally optimal for estimating treatment and carryover effects over the class of uniform designs. In a balanced design, each treatment is preceded by every other treatment an equal number of times, and a treatment can’t be preceded by itself. Balance makes the standard error of all pairs of treatment contrasts the same.

Cheng et al. (1980) introduced a new class of design called strongly balanced designs and proved that any strongly balanced uniform design is universally optimal for estimating direct and carryover effects over the entire class of designs d(t,p,g)d(t,p,g). A strongly balanced design is balanced with the relaxation that treatment can precede itself. If a design is strongly balanced, the number of groups (g) must be a multiple of t2t^{2} and p/t=m>1p/t=m>1. An example of a two-treatment strongly balanced design is ABBA-BAAB-AABB-BBAA (Kershner & Federer, 1981). This means when p>tp>t, these designs repeat the same treatment in consecutive periods. Kunert et al. (1984) worked with p=tp=t and claims that if there exists a BU design, then the ratio of the trace of the information matrices of BU design and any competing design has a lower bound of 1(t2t1)21-(t^{2}-t-1)^{-2}. This means that BU designs are nearly optimal for t>2t>2 but not strictly optimal. Kunert also proved that for t=2=pt=2=p, the optimal designs for estimating direct treatment and carryover effects are different, and none is uniform.

Sen & Mukerjee (1987) proved that when an interaction between treatment and carryover is present in the model, strongly balanced designs are still optimal for estimating direct treatment effects because they are orthogonal. Kempton et al. (2001) developed optimality results when carryover effects are proportional to treatment effects. Kunert & Stufken (2002) developed optimal designs in the presence of self and mixed carryover effects by introducing a new class of designs called totally balanced designs. When the objective is to compare a bunch of test treatments with a control treatment, Hedayat et al. (2005) obtained optimality results with p=t+1p=t+1, called extra-period designs. Many works have been done to find optimal designs when p<tp<t, we refer the reader to chapter 3 of Bose & Dey (2009). We conclude our discussion on optimal designs by making two points: First, one should prioritize the effects or the contrasts that are of primary concern before choosing a design. Second, in the possibility of subject drop-out, will the design still be nearly optimal if the experimenter has to stop the study before the last period? R-package Crossover (Rohmeyer, 2017) offers an excellent interface to choose between several designs under different carryover models by comparing the average efficiencies of the competing design under different correlation structures of error such as independence, compound symmetry (CS), etc.

4.3 Inference methods for continuous response

Depending on the design and the nature of experiments, the analyst might choose any model from (14) for the treatment and carryover effects with some baseline covariates recorded at the start of the study. For now, we assume that a single response is observed within a period for every subject. As stated before, crossover design with pp periods can be considered split-plot experiments where the whole-plots are the subjects, and measurements are observed within each subject at different periods. Standard ANOVA FF-test techniques for split-plots using random subject effect (whole plot error) can be conducted to test for treatment or carryover effects under a Gaussian assumption using proc mixed or R package lmerTest (Kuznetsova et al., 2017)

Alternatively, crossover design can also be viewed as a repeated measurements study where measurements are observed for each subject over consecutive periods. Defining, 𝐘ik=(Yi1k,,Yipk){\bm{\mathbf{{Y}}}}_{ik}=(Y_{i1k},\cdots,Y_{ipk})^{\top} as the vector of response for the kth subject in sequence ii, the random subject effect model in (3) induces an exchangeable or CS covariance structure for the response vector i.e. Var(𝐘ik)=𝚺=σ2𝐈p+σs2𝐉pi,k\textrm{Var}({\bm{\mathbf{{Y}}}}_{ik})={\bm{\mathbf{{\Sigma}}}}=\sigma^{2}{\bm{\mathbf{{I}}}}_{p}+\sigma_{s}^{2}{\bm{\mathbf{{J}}}}_{p}\forall\;i,k. However, this CS structure might be too parsimonious to capture the true covariance. To generalize this we specify the mean model as E(𝐘ik|𝐗)=𝐗ik𝜽E({\bm{\mathbf{{Y}}}}_{ik}\;|\;{\bm{\mathbf{{X}}}})={\bm{\mathbf{{X}}}}_{ik}{\bm{\mathbf{{\theta}}}}. The fixed parameters 𝜽{\bm{\mathbf{{\theta}}}} contain all the fixed parameters specified in the mean model assumed, and the design matrix 𝐗ik{\bm{\mathbf{{X}}}}_{ik} is specified accordingly. Furthermore, the covariance model is Var(𝐘ik|𝐗)=𝐕ik(ξ)\textrm{Var}({\bm{\mathbf{{Y}}}}_{ik}\;|\;{\bm{\mathbf{{X}}}})={\bm{\mathbf{{V}}}}_{ik}(\xi), which need not be compound symmetric. The vectors 𝜽{\bm{\mathbf{{\theta}}}} and 𝝃{\bm{\mathbf{{\xi}}}} are called mean and covariance parameters. This method directly captures the effect of treatment or carryover on the population mean response, which is called a population-averaged (PA) model. The influence of any random subject effect is captured in the complex covariance matrix. We assume that the responses for different subjects are mutually uncorrelated.

Under Gaussian assumption, the mean parameter 𝜽{\bm{\mathbf{{\theta}}}} is estimated through the generalized least square (GLS) approach and covariance parameters can be estimated using the restricted maximum likelihood (REML) method to adjust for the bias in the estimation of covariance. If normality is not assumed, then the mean component is estimated using linear estimating equations. SAS procedure proc mixed, proc glimmix and R-package nlme (Pinheiro et al., 2018) offers flexible PA models. The covariance structure can be varied according to any baseline covariate, e.g., the subject’s sex. Many different covariance matrix structures can be assumed, such as Autoregressive (AR), CS, Ante, and Unstructured (Un), the most general one. The final choice of the covariance model is made by AIC or BIC criterion. If the model is correctly specified, then GLS estimator of 𝜽{\bm{\mathbf{{\theta}}}} with its asymptotic distribution is given by

𝜽^=(i,k𝐗ik𝐕ik1(ξ^)𝐗ik)1i,k𝐗ik𝐕ik1(ξ^)𝐘ikAsymptotically N(𝜽,𝚺^M)\displaystyle\hat{{\bm{\mathbf{{\theta}}}}}=\left(\sum_{i,k}{\bm{\mathbf{{X}}}}_{ik}^{\top}{\bm{\mathbf{{V}}}}_{ik}^{-1}(\hat{\xi}){\bm{\mathbf{{X}}}}_{ik}\right)^{-1}\sum_{i,k}{\bm{\mathbf{{X}}}}_{ik}^{\top}{\bm{\mathbf{{V}}}}_{ik}^{-1}(\hat{\xi}){\bm{\mathbf{{Y}}}}_{ik}\sim\textrm{Asymptotically N}\left({\bm{\mathbf{{\theta}}}},\hat{{\bm{\mathbf{{\Sigma}}}}}_{M}\right) (15)

where ξ^\hat{\xi} is the REML/ML estimator of ξ\xi and 𝚺M^=(i,k𝐗ik𝐕ik1(ξ^)𝐗ik)1\hat{{\bm{\mathbf{{\Sigma}}}}_{M}}=\left(\sum_{i,k}{\bm{\mathbf{{X}}}}_{ik}^{\top}{\bm{\mathbf{{V}}}}_{ik}^{-1}(\hat{\xi}){\bm{\mathbf{{X}}}}_{ik}\right)^{-1} is called the model-based SE. If the sample size in each group is not large, adjustment due to Kenward & Roger (1997) can be done. However, this adjustment still understates the SE of the estimator if the covariance structure is incorrectly specified. Diggle et al. (2002) pointed out in chapter 4 that the assumption of diagonal covariance does not affect the estimator of the mean parameter 𝜽{\bm{\mathbf{{\theta}}}}. Still, it overstates or understates the SE of the estimator. The robust or empirical SE introduced in the context of non-normal response using a generalized estimating equation (GEE) gives protection against wrongly specified covariance (Zeger et al., 1988). The authors proved that under certain assumptions, as long as the mean model is correctly specified, the GLS estimator of the mean parameter still follows an asymptotic normal distribution with the covariance matrix replaced by 𝚺^R\hat{{\bm{\mathbf{{\Sigma}}}}}_{R} (Yuan & Jennrich, 1998), which is greater than the model-based SE (in the positive definite sense). The empirical option in SAS proc mixed allows inference on mean parameters using robust standard error. Adjustment to the empirical SE due to Mancl & DeRouen (2001) can be done for a small sample. Wald-type test for testing H0:𝐂𝜽=ϕH_{0}:{\bm{\mathbf{{C}}}}{\bm{\mathbf{{\theta}}}}=\phi and approximate CI can be constructed using the test-statistic

T=(𝐂𝜽^ϕ)(𝐂𝚺^𝐂)1(𝐂𝜽^ϕ)χr2asymptotically;r=rank(𝐂)\displaystyle T=\left({\bm{\mathbf{{C}}}}\hat{{\bm{\mathbf{{\theta}}}}}-\phi\right)^{\top}\left({\bm{\mathbf{{C}}}}\hat{{\bm{\mathbf{{\Sigma}}}}}{\bm{\mathbf{{C}}}}^{\top}\right)^{-1}\left({\bm{\mathbf{{C}}}}\hat{{\bm{\mathbf{{\theta}}}}}-\phi\right)\sim\chi^{2}_{r}\quad\textrm{asymptotically}\;\;;\quad\;\;r=\textrm{rank}({\bm{\mathbf{{C}}}}) (16)

If the primary questions of interest are to investigate the effect of treatment on each subject, then a subject-specific (SS) linear mixed effect model is appropriate. If 𝐛{\bm{\mathbf{{b}}}} is the vector of random effects then a SS model can be written as E(𝐘ik|𝐗,𝐛)=𝐗ik𝜽+𝐙ik𝐛E({\bm{\mathbf{{Y}}}}_{ik}\;|\;{\bm{\mathbf{{X}}}},{\bm{\mathbf{{b}}}})={\bm{\mathbf{{X}}}}_{ik}{\bm{\mathbf{{\theta}}}}+{\bm{\mathbf{{Z}}}}_{ik}{\bm{\mathbf{{b}}}}. Usually, 𝐛{\bm{\mathbf{{b}}}} is assumed to follow Gaussian with mean 0 and unknown covariance 𝐃{\bm{\mathbf{{D}}}}. The PA model that generates from the SS model by taking an expectation over 𝐛{\bm{\mathbf{{b}}}} is E(𝐘ik|𝐗)=𝐗ik𝜽E({\bm{\mathbf{{Y}}}}_{ik}\;|\;{\bm{\mathbf{{X}}}})={\bm{\mathbf{{X}}}}_{ik}{\bm{\mathbf{{\theta}}}}, Var(𝐘ik|𝐗)=𝐙ik𝐃𝐙ik+𝐑ik\textrm{Var}({\bm{\mathbf{{Y}}}}_{ik}\;|\;{\bm{\mathbf{{X}}}})={\bm{\mathbf{{Z}}}}_{ik}{\bm{\mathbf{{D}}}}{\bm{\mathbf{{Z}}}}_{ik}^{\top}+{\bm{\mathbf{{R}}}}_{ik}, where 𝐑ik{\bm{\mathbf{{R}}}}_{ik} is the covariance of the measurement error, possibly diagonal. This generates an equivalent PA model. However, in pharmacokinetic studies or when the carryover effect is assumed to be proportional to the treatment effect, the model becomes non-linear (Kempton et al., 2001). An SS model can be defined as E(𝐘ik|𝐗,𝐛)=𝐟(𝐙ik,𝜽,𝐛)E({\bm{\mathbf{{Y}}}}_{ik}\;|\;{\bm{\mathbf{{X}}}},{\bm{\mathbf{{b}}}})={\bm{\mathbf{{f}}}}({\bm{\mathbf{{Z}}}}_{ik},{\bm{\mathbf{{\theta}}}},{\bm{\mathbf{{b}}}}) and an appropriate SS covariance model with parameter 𝝍{\bm{\mathbf{{\psi}}}}. A PA model can be derived from this by integrating the likelihood over the distribution of 𝐛{\bm{\mathbf{{b}}}}. Numerical approximations such as the adaptive quadrature method, a first-order approximation of likelihood are typically used to approximate the marginal distribution, and the estimation of 𝜽{\bm{\mathbf{{\theta}}}} and 𝝃{\bm{\mathbf{{\xi}}}} is carried out through the GEE method or maximizing the approximated likelihood directly. See Davidian & Giltinan (2003) for a series of methodologies available in this context. SAS procedure proc nlmixed implements this method with great efficiency.

5 Using baseline measurements : AB-BA design revisited

The test for the carryover effect is based on subject totals and lacks power due to the high between-subject variability. Consequently, the PAR test for treatment effect becomes inefficient. Hills & Armitage (1979) proposed using baseline measurements collected before the start of each period in the study to potentially improve the power of the tests described in Section 3. In many real applications, baseline measurements are collected only before the start of the first period. Using the baseline observations as responses, we can interpret the study as a four-period, two-treatment crossover trial. Moreover, it is intuitive to include an effect(θ)(\theta) that differentiates between the two baseline responses due to the effect of the treatment in period-1, apart from the usual period(π\pi), direct treatment(τ)(\tau) and carryover effect (λ)(\lambda). Analysis can be done using the methods described in Section 4.3 by specifying a suitable covariance structure for the four-variate response. In the presence of a significant λ\lambda and θ\theta, the direct treatment effect is estimated using the difference between the first baseline and first period responses. If the model is specified correctly, this approach can provide a powerful test for the treatment effect as the between-subject variability is eliminated. The difference between the two baseline responses estimates θ\theta(Patel, 1983). When both λ\lambda and θ\theta are null, this method produces the same within-subject estimator for τ\tau as in (5). Non-parametric analysis can also be done following the rank-measure-based method by Jung & Koch (1999).

While many people adopted this procedure to estimate treatment effect with more power, Fleiss et al. (1985) presented a very critical review on using this method by showcasing that if θ\theta behaves proportionally to the treatment effect τ\tau, then an induced or hypothetical carryover might appear even if it is genuinely insignificant. Subsequently, the inference drawn from the method can go wrong. Willian & Pater (1986) considered a random subject by period interaction and concluded that using baseline measurements can significantly diminish the power and precision if the subject by period variation is less than the within-subject variation, which is often the case in real-life applications, and this method should be applied with caution.

6 Analysis of Multivariate Continuous Response

A multivariate response can arise in multiple practical applications. 1) The response that is of interest is inherently multivariate, and a single observation is recorded within each period. 2) The response might be univariate, but repeated measurements will be recorded within each period. 3) The response is multivariate and repeated measurements are observed within a period. Wallenstein (1979) considered a split-plot model when the response is single variate repeatedly measured over time within each period. The author considered a random effect structure for the subject by time interaction and analyzed the model using a univariate ANOVA approach. However, the model induces a CS structure for the response vector. Patel & Hearne III (1980) considered a common unstructured covariance for each response and analyzed through Wilk’s lambda test-statistic. Johnson (1996) in his Ph.D. thesis, developed a general multivariate ANOVA procedure to test for any effect in an AB-BA crossover design, considering that the multivariate response can occur from any of the three cases stated above. To give an overview of the approach, let E(𝐘ijk)=𝝁ijE({\bm{\mathbf{{Y}}}}_{ijk})={\bm{\mathbf{{\mu}}}}_{ij} be the respective mean vector for the response vector at period jj of the group ii. Defining the matrix of mean vectors =((𝝁ij))\mathcal{M}=\left(({\bm{\mathbf{{\mu}}}}_{ij})\right), any hypothesis of fixed effects can be written as H0:𝐂𝐔=𝟎H_{0}:{\bm{\mathbf{{C}}}}\mathcal{M}{\bm{\mathbf{{U}}}}={\bm{\mathbf{{0}}}}, for appropriate coefficient matrices 𝐂{\bm{\mathbf{{C}}}} and 𝐔{\bm{\mathbf{{U}}}}. The likelihood ratio test has been proposed to test for different fixed effects. The test statistics generally follow an F-distribution with appropriate degrees of freedom. Johnson & Grender (1993) proposed a non-parametric test using a multivariate generalization of Wilcoxon rank-sum statistic.

If a univariate response is recorded repeatedly over time within a period, then the ANOVA methods described above consider time as a factor and require the data to be balanced. To overcome this and to characterize the pattern of the response profile as a smooth function of time, a PA or an SS approach that models the response as a linear or polynomial function of time can be more appealing (chapter 5 of Jones & Kenward 2014). Inference for fixed effects can also be carried through the results described in Section 4.3. Recently, Kawaguchi & Koch (2010) developed a multivariate Mann-Whitney type non-parametric approach for a multivariate ordinal response following Jung & Koch (1999) under a two-period three-treatment crossover design.

7 Analysis of Binary/Categorical response

In many real-life applications, the response can be categorical, i.e., the response can take only a few categories. See Senn (2002), Jones & Kenward (2014) for example. Binary response occurs when the response can assume only two categories. This section will review the inference methodologies developed for binary and categorical responses under a crossover design. First, we start with the AB-BA design. We stick to the earlier notation for a typical response, but we say Yijk=1Y_{ijk}=1 if the response for kkth subject in iith group in period jj is a success and 0 if it is a failure. For example, suppose we want to compare the efficiency of a drug (A) for cerebrovascular deficiency to a placebo (B). The response is the indicator variable denoting whether the cardiologist considers the electrocardiogram Normal (success) or abnormal (failure). Using the crossover difference CikC_{ik} in (2), define

Ni=k=1ni𝕀(|Cik|=1);NiA=k=1ni𝕀(Cik=1);NiB=k=1ni𝕀(Cik=1)\displaystyle N_{i}=\sum_{k=1}^{n_{i}}\mathbb{I}(\lvert C_{ik}\rvert=1)\;;\quad N_{i}^{A}=\sum_{k=1}^{n_{i}}\mathbb{I}(C_{ik}=1)\;;\quad N_{i}^{B}=\sum_{k=1}^{n_{i}}\mathbb{I}(C_{ik}=-1) (17)

Then, NiN_{i} is the total number of subjects in sequence ii who show any preference to A or B. Out of them, NiAN_{i}^{A} showed a preference for A, and NiBN_{i}^{B} showed a preference for B. To illustrate the term preference, we say that a preference occurs if the responses for a subject under treatment A and B differ. Moreover, there is a preference towards treatment A if the response under treatment A is a success and that under treatment B is a failure, the vice-versa for the interpretation of preference towards treatment B. For now, we assume that there is no carryover effect. Under the assumption that there is no period effect (π=0\pi=0), McNemar (1947)’s test for paired responses can be adapted to test for treatment effects. Because, under H0:τ=0H_{0}:\tau=0, N1A+N2AN_{1}^{A}+N_{2}^{A} follows binomial with parameter N1+N2N_{1}+N_{2} and 0.50.5. Then, we can either do an exact or approximate χ2\chi^{2} test with one degree of freedom depending on the sample size.

McNemar’s test does not account for the period effect, which is often significant in crossover designs. Mainland (1964) proposed a heuristic test for treatment that adjusts for the period effect based on the association between preference to the treatments and the groups. If τ=0\tau=0, then the odds in favor of treatment A to B in group 1 should be the same as the odds in favor of treatment A in group 2. A chi-square test of independence or Fisher’s exact test in a 2×22\times 2 contingency table with one d.f can be constructed. Gart (1969) provides a theoretical justification of the test using a logistic model conditional on (fixed) subject effects. Mainland-Gart and McNemar tests discard the responses when |Vik|=0\lvert V_{ik}\rvert=0, i.e., the responses which show no preference to any treatment. Prescott (1981) proposed a test which is similar to Tudor & Koch (1994) approach described in 3.3. It considers a 2×32\times 3 contingency table where the extra columns include the responses that show no preferences. This gives a chi-square test with 22 degrees of freedom asymptotically. The simulation studies suggest that in the absence of the period effect, the McNemar and Prescott tests are more powerful than the Mainland-Gart test in detecting the treatment effect. The Prescott test performs better than both in the presence of a period effect.

All the methodologies discussed above were developed in terms of a contingency table and are useful for testing effects specifically under AB-BA design. Parametric marginal models have also been proposed for binary response under higher-order crossover designs. Unlike the PA model for a continuous response, the mean and covariance parameters are functionally related for a binary response. Following Diggle et al. (2002), we write the population-averaged generalized linear model as follows

P(Yijk=1)=E(Yijk)=𝐟(𝐱ijk,𝜽)Var(Yijk)=𝐟(𝐱ijk,𝜽)(1𝐟(𝐱ijk,𝜽))=vijk(𝜽)\displaystyle P(Y_{ijk}=1)=E(Y_{ijk})={\bm{\mathbf{{f}}}}\left({\bm{\mathbf{{x}}}}_{ijk},{\bm{\mathbf{{\theta}}}}\right)\quad\textrm{Var}(Y_{ijk})={\bm{\mathbf{{f}}}}\left({\bm{\mathbf{{x}}}}_{ijk},{\bm{\mathbf{{\theta}}}}\right)(1-{\bm{\mathbf{{f}}}}\left({\bm{\mathbf{{x}}}}_{ijk},{\bm{\mathbf{{\theta}}}}\right))=v_{ijk}({\bm{\mathbf{{\theta}}}}) (18)

where 𝐟{\bm{\mathbf{{f}}}} is an appropriate link function that maps onto (0,1)(0,1). In most of cases, 𝐟(𝐱ijk,𝜽)=1/(1+exp(𝐱ijk𝜽)){\bm{\mathbf{{f}}}}\left({\bm{\mathbf{{x}}}}_{ijk},{\bm{\mathbf{{\theta}}}}\right)=1/(1+\exp(-{\bm{\mathbf{{x}}}}_{ijk}^{\top}{\bm{\mathbf{{\theta}}}})). However, the specification of the marginal variance is insufficient to capture the correlation between the two responses for a subject. Also, for binary responses, the parameter space of the correlation is restricted. In the literature, it is standard to assume a working correlation structure. Assuming a working correlation matrix 𝚪(𝜶,𝐱ik){\bm{\mathbf{{\Gamma}}}}({\bm{\mathbf{{\alpha}}}},{\bm{\mathbf{{x}}}}_{ik}) for 𝐘ik{\bm{\mathbf{{Y}}}}_{ik}, we get the marginal covariance matrix as Var(𝐘ik)=𝐕ik(𝜽,𝜶)=[Diag((vijk))]1/2Γ(α,𝐱ik)[Diag((vijk))]1/2\textrm{Var}({\bm{\mathbf{{Y}}}}_{ik})={\bm{\mathbf{{V}}}}_{ik}({\bm{\mathbf{{\theta}}}},{\bm{\mathbf{{\alpha}}}})=\left[\textrm{Diag}((v_{ijk}))\right]^{1/2}\Gamma(\alpha,{\bm{\mathbf{{x}}}}_{ik})\left[\textrm{Diag}((v_{ijk}))\right]^{1/2}. Consistent estimation of 𝜽{\bm{\mathbf{{\theta}}}} and 𝜶{\bm{\mathbf{{\alpha}}}} can be done by GEE (Liang & Zeger, 1986) or quadratic estimating equation (GEE-2, Prentice & Zhao 1991). Under certain technical assumptions, the large sample distribution of 𝜽{\bm{\mathbf{{\theta}}}} has the same form as we obtained in (15), provided the mean model is correctly specified. In practice, the covariance structure is incorrectly specified, and a robust covariance matrix is used to draw inferences about 𝜽{\bm{\mathbf{{\theta}}}}. This method is implemented in SAS procedure proc genmod, proc gee, proc glimmix and geepack in R((Højsgaard et al., 2006)).

Similarly, allowing for a subject-specific random effect, a generalized linear mixed model (GLMM) for binary response can be defined as follows,

E(𝐘ik|𝐒ik)=𝐟(𝐱ik,𝜽,𝐒ik)Var(𝐘ik|𝐒ik)=𝐑ik(𝜽,𝜶,𝐱ik,𝐒ik)\displaystyle E({\bm{\mathbf{{Y}}}}_{ik}|{\bm{\mathbf{{S}}}}_{ik})={\bm{\mathbf{{f}}}}\left({\bm{\mathbf{{x}}}}_{ik},{\bm{\mathbf{{\theta}}}},{\bm{\mathbf{{S}}}}_{ik}\right)\quad\textrm{Var}({\bm{\mathbf{{Y}}}}_{ik}|{\bm{\mathbf{{S}}}}_{ik})={\bm{\mathbf{{R}}}}_{ik}({\bm{\mathbf{{\theta}}}},{\bm{\mathbf{{\alpha}}}},{\bm{\mathbf{{x}}}}_{ik},{\bm{\mathbf{{S}}}}_{ik}) (19)

In general, it is assumed that given 𝐒ik{\bm{\mathbf{{S}}}}_{ik}, the observations in each period are independently distributed so that 𝐑ik{\bm{\mathbf{{R}}}}_{ik} be diagonal. Then, the unconditional mean and covariance model for 𝐘ik{\bm{\mathbf{{Y}}}}_{ik} can be obtained by integrating over the distribution of 𝐒ik{\bm{\mathbf{{S}}}}_{ik}, which is also usually assumed to be Gaussian. Large sample inference for 𝜽{\bm{\mathbf{{\theta}}}} can be carried out through first-order approximation, GEE, or Bayesian methods, implemented in SAS procedure proc glimmix. See Davidian & Giltinan (2003) for details.

If the response assumes M (>2)(>2) categories, then the response can be transformed to 𝐘ijk=𝐞m{\bm{\mathbf{{Y}}}}_{ijk}={\bm{\mathbf{{e}}}}_{m} if the response is in the mmth category. Where 𝐞mM{\bm{\mathbf{{e}}}}_{m}\in\mathbb{R}^{M} be the unit vector, m=1,,Mm=1,\dots,M. Let πijk,m=P(𝐘ijk=𝐞m)\pi_{ijk,m}=P({\bm{\mathbf{{Y}}}}_{ijk}={\bm{\mathbf{{e}}}}_{m}). Analogous to the extension of binary logistic regression to multinomial logistic regression, generalized logit model πijk,m/πijk,1=𝐟(𝐱ik,𝜽m)m=2,,M\pi_{ijk,m}/\pi_{ijk,1}={\bm{\mathbf{{f}}}}\left({\bm{\mathbf{{x}}}}_{ik},{\bm{\mathbf{{\theta}}}}_{m}\right)\;\;m=2,\dots,M. SAS procedure proc genmod can fit a population average logit model for categorical response. If the response is ordinal, then the proportional odds model can also be fit that characterizes P(Yijkm),m=1,,MP(Y_{ijk}\geq m),m=1,\dots,M through suitable link function and fixed effect parameters. See Jones & Kenward (2014) for further details.

8 Conclusion and Open problems

Statistical methods developed for longitudinal study (Diggle et al., 2002) can also be applied to any crossover design. In the AB-BA design, the carryover causes a nuisance to the efficient estimation of direct treatment. Including a sufficiently long washout period can be effective in this situation. No particular procedure discussed in Section 3.1 outperforms the other in all scenarios. We shortlist some open problems in the literature. Firstly, as a crossover study with more than two periods lasts long or multiple treatments are given on the subject in different periods, subject drop-out is inevitable. A significant amount of work is still to be done when the assumption of missing at random (MAR) is invalidated. Secondly, non-parametric or semi-parametric estimation of functional response profile in the context of repeated measurements within a period, along with the asymptotic properties, is yet to be explored. Thirdly, from a design perspective, if the carryover is proportional to direct treatment, then the optimal design developed assumes that the proportionality parameter is known. The construction of a robust design for the correct estimation of the unknown parameter might be essential. Lastly, one of the primary advantages of the crossover design is that it requires a comparably small number of subjects in the analysis. However, the small sample properties of the estimators we discussed are still to be examined rigorously.

References

  • (1)
  • Afsarinejad & Hedayat (2002) Afsarinejad, K. & Hedayat, A. (2002), ‘Repeated measurements designs for a model with self and simple mixed carryover effects’, Journal of Statistical Planning and Inference 106(1-2), 449–459.
  • Armitage & Hills (1982) Armitage, P. & Hills, M. (1982), ‘The two-period crossover trial’, Journal of the Royal Statistical Society: Series D (The Statistician) 31(2), 119–131.
  • Bose & Dey (2009) Bose, M. & Dey, A. (2009), Optimal crossover designs, World Scientific.
  • Brown Jr (1980) Brown Jr, B. W. (1980), ‘The crossover experiment for clinical trials’, Biometrics pp. 69–79.
  • Chatterjee & Sen (1964) Chatterjee, S. K. & Sen, P. K. (1964), ‘Non-parametric tests for the bivariate two-sample location problem’, Calcutta Statistical Association Bulletin 13(1-2), 18–58.
  • Cheng et al. (1980) Cheng, C.-S., Wu, C.-F. et al. (1980), ‘Balanced repeated measurements designs’, The Annals of Statistics 8(6), 1272–1283.
  • Cochran et al. (1941) Cochran, W., Autrey, K. & Cannon, C. (1941), ‘A double change-over design for dairy cattle feeding experiments’, Journal of Dairy Science 24(11), 937–951.
  • Cornell (1991) Cornell, R. G. (1991), ‘Nonparametric tests of dispersion for the two-period crossover designo’, Communications in Statistics-Theory and Methods 20(3), 1099–1106.
  • Cox (1958) Cox, D. R. (1958), ‘Planning of experiments.’.
  • David & Seaman Jr (2002) David, S. & Seaman Jr, J. (2002), ‘Hierarchical bayesian analysis of the carryover effect in two-period crossover designs’, Mathematical and computer modelling 36(1-2), 129–138.
  • Davidian & Giltinan (2003) Davidian, M. & Giltinan, D. M. (2003), ‘Nonlinear models for repeated measurement data: an overview and update’, Journal of agricultural, biological, and environmental statistics 8(4), 387.
  • Diggle et al. (2002) Diggle, P., Diggle, P. J., Heagerty, P., Heagerty, P. J., Liang, K.-Y., Zeger, S. et al. (2002), Analysis of longitudinal data, Oxford University Press.
  • Finney (1956) Finney, D. J. (1956), ‘Cross-over designs in bioassay’, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B-Biological Sciences 145(918), 42–61.
  • Fleiss et al. (1985) Fleiss, J. L., Wallenstein, S. & Rosenfeld, R. (1985), ‘Adjusting for baseline measurements in the two-period crossover study: A cautionary note’, Controlled Clinical Trials 6(3), 192–197.
  • Freeman (1989) Freeman, P. (1989), ‘The performance of the two-stage analysis of two-treatment, two-period crossover trials’, Statistics in medicine 8(12), 1421–1432.
  • Gart (1969) Gart, J. J. (1969), ‘An exact test for comparing matched proportions in crossover designs’, Biometrika 56(1), 75–80.
  • Grieve (1985) Grieve, A. (1985), ‘A bayesian analysis of the two-period crossover design for clinical trials.’, Biometrics 41(4), 979–990.
  • Grizzle (1965) Grizzle, J. E. (1965), ‘The two-period change-over design and its use in clinical trials’, Biometrics pp. 467–480.
  • Harker et al. (2008) Harker, F. R., Kupferman, E. M., Marin, A. B., Gunson, F. A. & Triggs, C. M. (2008), ‘Eating quality standards for apples based on consumer preferences’, Postharvest biology and technology 50(1), 70–78.
  • Hedayat et al. (1978) Hedayat, A., Afsarinejad, K. et al. (1978), ‘Repeated measurements designs, ii’, The Annals of Statistics 6(3), 619–628.
  • Hedayat et al. (2005) Hedayat, A., Yang, M. et al. (2005), ‘Optimal and efficient crossover designs for comparing test treatments with a control treatment’, The Annals of Statistics 33(2), 915–943.
  • Hills & Armitage (1979) Hills, M. & Armitage, P. (1979), ‘The two-period cross-over clinical trial.’, British journal of clinical pharmacology 8(1), 7–20.
  • Hodges Jr & Lehmann (1963) Hodges Jr, J. L. & Lehmann, E. L. (1963), ‘Estimates of location based on rank tests’, The Annals of Mathematical Statistics pp. 598–611.
  • Højsgaard et al. (2006) Højsgaard, S., Halekoh, U. & Yan, J. (2006), ‘The r package geepack for generalized estimating equations’, Journal of Statistical Software 15/2, 1–11.
  • Jaki et al. (2013) Jaki, T., Pallmann, P. & Wolfsegger, M. J. (2013), ‘Estimation in ab/ba crossover trials with application to bioequivalence studies with incomplete and complete data designs’, Statistics in medicine 32(30), 5469–5483.
  • Johnson (1996) Johnson, W. D. (1996), Multivariate models for the analysis of crossover designs, PhD thesis, Tulane University.
  • Johnson & Grender (1993) Johnson, W. D. & Grender, J. M. (1993), ‘Multivariate nonparametric analysis for the two-period crossover design with application in clinical trials’, Journal of biopharmaceutical statistics 3(1), 1–12.
  • Jones & Kenward (2014) Jones, B. & Kenward, M. G. (2014), Design and analysis of cross-over trials, CRC press.
  • Jung & Koch (1999) Jung, J.-W. & Koch, G. G. (1999), ‘Multivariate non-parametric methods for mann–whitney statistics to analyse cross-over studies with two treatment sequences’, Statistics in medicine 18(8), 989–1017.
  • Kawaguchi & Koch (2010) Kawaguchi, A. & Koch, G. G. (2010), ‘Multivariate mann–whitney estimators for the comparison of two treatments in a three-period crossover study with randomly missing data’, Journal of biopharmaceutical statistics 20(4), 720–744.
  • Kempton et al. (2001) Kempton, R., Ferris, S. & David, O. (2001), ‘Optimal change-over designs when carry-over effects are proportional to direct effects of treatments’, Biometrika 88(2), 391–399.
  • Kenward & Roger (1997) Kenward, M. G. & Roger, J. H. (1997), ‘Small sample inference for fixed effects from restricted maximum likelihood’, Biometrics pp. 983–997.
  • Kershner & Federer (1981) Kershner, R. P. & Federer, W. T. (1981), ‘Two-treatment crossover designs for estimating a variety of effects’, Journal of the American Statistical Association 76(375), 612–619.
  • Kiefer (1975) Kiefer, J. (1975), ‘Construction and optimality of generalized youden designs ii’, A Survey of Statistical Designs and Linear Models pp. 333–353.
  • Koch (1972) Koch, G. G. (1972), ‘328. note: The use of none-parametric methods in the statistical analysis of the two-period change-over design’, Biometrics pp. 577–584.
  • Kunert & Stufken (2002) Kunert, J. & Stufken, J. (2002), ‘Optimal crossover designs in a model with self and mixed carryover effects’, Journal of the American Statistical Association 97(459), 898–906.
  • Kunert et al. (1984) Kunert, J. et al. (1984), ‘Optimality of balanced uniform repeated measurements designs’, The Annals of Statistics 12(3), 1006–1017.
  • Kuznetsova et al. (2017) Kuznetsova, A., Brockhoff, P. B. & Christensen, R. H. B. (2017), ‘lmerTest package: Tests in linear mixed effects models’, Journal of Statistical Software 82(13), 1–26.
  • Li & Sivaganesan (2017) Li, D. & Sivaganesan, S. (2017), ‘An objective bayesian estimation in a two-period crossover design’, Communications in Statistics-Theory and Methods 46(15), 7409–7426.
  • Liang & Zeger (1986) Liang, K.-Y. & Zeger, S. L. (1986), ‘Longitudinal data analysis using generalized linear models’, Biometrika 73(1), 13–22.
  • Liebig et al. (1847) Liebig, J., Gregory, W. et al. (1847), ‘Researches on the chemistry of food’.
  • Lui (2016) Lui, K.-J. (2016), Crossover designs: Testing, estimation, and sample size, John Wiley & Sons.
  • Mainland (1964) Mainland, D. (1964), ‘Elementary medical statistics’, Academic Medicine 39(3), 331.
  • Mancl & DeRouen (2001) Mancl, L. A. & DeRouen, T. A. (2001), ‘A covariance estimator for gee with improved small-sample properties’, Biometrics 57(1), 126–134.
  • Mann & Whitney (1947) Mann, H. B. & Whitney, D. R. (1947), ‘On a test of whether one of two random variables is stochastically larger than the other’, The annals of mathematical statistics pp. 50–60.
  • McNemar (1947) McNemar, Q. (1947), ‘Note on the sampling error of the difference between correlated proportions or percentages’, Psychometrika 12(2), 153–157.
  • Moran (1959) Moran, P. (1959), ‘The power of a cross-over test for the artificial stimulation of rain’, Australian Journal of Statistics 1(2), 47–52.
  • Ozan & Stufken (2010) Ozan, M. O. & Stufken, J. (2010), ‘Assessing the impact of carryover effects on the variances of estimators of treatment differences in crossover designs’, Statistics in medicine 29(24), 2480–2485.
  • Patel (1983) Patel, H. (1983), ‘Use of baseline measurements in the two-period crossover design’, Communications in Statistics-Theory and Methods 12(23), 2693–2712.
  • Patel & Hearne III (1980) Patel, H. & Hearne III, E. M. (1980), ‘Multivariate analysis for the two-period repeated measures crossover design with application to clinical trials’, Communications in Statistics-Theory and Methods 9(18), 1919–1929.
  • Pinheiro et al. (2018) Pinheiro, J., Bates, D., DebRoy, S., Sarkar, D. & R Core Team (2018), nlme: Linear and Nonlinear Mixed Effects Models. R package version 3.1-137.
    https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=nlme
  • Prentice & Zhao (1991) Prentice, R. L. & Zhao, L. P. (1991), ‘Estimating equations for parameters in means and covariances of multivariate discrete and continuous responses’, Biometrics pp. 825–839.
  • Prescott (1981) Prescott, R. (1981), ‘The comparison of success rates in cross-over trials in the presence of an order effect’, Journal of the Royal Statistical Society: Series C (Applied Statistics) 30(1), 9–15.
  • R Core Team (2018) R Core Team (2018), R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing, R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria.
    https://www.R-project.org/
  • Ratkowsky et al. (1992) Ratkowsky, D., Alldredge, R. & Evans, M. A. (1992), Cross-over experiments: design, analysis and application, Vol. 135, CRC Press.
  • Rohmeyer (2017) Rohmeyer, K. (2017), Crossover: Analysis and Search of Crossover Designs. R package version 0.1-17.
    https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=Crossover
  • Ruiz-Cantero et al. (2007) Ruiz-Cantero, M. T., Vives-Cases, C., Artazcoz, L., Delgado, A., Calvente, M. d. M. G., Miqueo, C., Montero, I., Ortiz, R., Ronda, E., Ruiz, I. et al. (2007), ‘A framework to analyse gender bias in epidemiological research’, Journal of Epidemiology & Community Health 61(Suppl 2), ii46–ii53.
  • SAS Institute Inc (2008) SAS Institute Inc (2008), ‘SAS/STAT® 9.2 User’s Guide. Cary, NC: SAS Institute Inc’.
  • Selwyn et al. (1981) Selwyn, M. R., Dempster, A. P. & Hall, N. R. (1981), ‘A bayesian approach to bioequivalence for the 2 x 2 changeover design.’, Biometrics 37(1), 11–21.
  • Sen & Mukerjee (1987) Sen, M. & Mukerjee, R. (1987), ‘Optimal repeated measurements designs under interaction’, Journal of Statistical Planning and Inference 17, 81–91.
  • Senn (1996) Senn, S. (1996), ‘The ab/ba cross-over: how to perform the two-stage analysis if you can’t be persuaded that you shouldn’t’, Liber Amicorum Roel van Strik pp. 93–100.
  • Senn (2002) Senn, S. (2002), Cross-over trials in clinical research, Vol. 5, John Wiley & Sons.
  • Sheehe (1961) Sheehe, P. R. (1961), ‘Latin squares to balance immediate residual, and other order, effects’, Biometrics 17(3), 405–414.
  • Tudor & Koch (1994) Tudor, G. & Koch, G. G. (1994), ‘Review of nonparametric methods for the analysis of crossover studies’, Statistical Methods in Medical Research 3(4), 345–381.
  • Wallenstein (1979) Wallenstein, S. (1979), Inclusion of baseline values in the analysis of crossover designs, in ‘Biometrics’, Vol. 35, INTERNATIONAL BIOMETRIC SOC 808 17TH ST NW SUITE 200, WASHINGTON, DC 20006-3910, pp. 894–894.
  • Wang & Hung (1997) Wang, S.-J. & Hung, H. J. (1997), ‘Use of two-stage test statistic in the two-period crossover trials’, Biometrics pp. 1081–1091.
  • Willan (1988) Willan, A. R. (1988), ‘Using the maximum test statistic in the two-period crossover clinical trial’, Biometrics pp. 211–218.
  • Willan & Pater (1986) Willan, A. R. & Pater, J. L. (1986), ‘Carryover and the two-period crossover clinical trial’, Biometrics pp. 593–599.
  • Willian & Pater (1986) Willian, A. R. & Pater, J. L. (1986), ‘Using baseline measurements in the two-period crossover clinical trial’, Controlled Clinical Trials 7(4), 282–289.
  • Yuan & Jennrich (1998) Yuan, K.-H. & Jennrich, R. I. (1998), ‘Asymptotics of estimating equations under natural conditions’, Journal of Multivariate Analysis 65(2), 245–260.
  • Zeger et al. (1988) Zeger, S. L., Liang, K.-Y. & Albert, P. S. (1988), ‘Models for longitudinal data: a generalized estimating equation approach’, Biometrics pp. 1049–1060.
  • Zellner & Siow (1980) Zellner, A. & Siow, A. (1980), ‘Posterior odds ratios for selected regression hypotheses’, Trabajos de estadística y de investigación operativa 31(1), 585–603.